Athletes’ Self-Reports on Mind Wandering While Practicing Sports: An Exploratory Two-Study Project

in Journal of Clinical Sport Psychology

Click name to view affiliation

Alexander T. LatinjakUniversity of Suffolk

Search for other papers by Alexander T. Latinjak in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
*
Restricted access

This two-study project provided a brief description of athletes’ experiences with mind wandering. Study 1 aimed to quantitatively examine mind wandering in sports, in terms of frequency, effects and perceived control. Therefore, 94 athletes (Mage = 19.51, SD = 1.65) answered a specifically designed 19-item questionnaire. The results suggested that mind wandering is a common phenomenon in sports, with both beneficial and adverse effects on performance. Study 2 aimed to qualitatively explore when athletes use mind wandering. Accordingly, 115 athletes (Mage = 22.82, SD = 3.61) described one recent mind wandering situation while practicing sport. A hierarchical content analysis was performed by the first author and confirmed by an external expert. The results indicated that mind wandering occurred in a wide range of situations in sport and physical activity. Nonetheless, it was also argued that future studies should more carefully define mind wandering to avoid confusion with related terms.

Latinjak is with the School for Science, Technology and Engineering, University of Suffolk, Ipswich, United Kingdom.

Address author correspondence to Alexander T. Latinjak at a.latinjak@uos.ac.uk.
  • Collapse
  • Expand
  • Álvarez M., Balaguer I., Castillo I., & Duda J.L. (2009). Coach autonomy support and quality of sport engagement in young soccer players. The Spanish Journal of Psychology, 12, 138148. PubMed ID: 19476227 doi:10.1017/S1138741600001554

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Birrer D., & Morgan G. (2010). Psychological skills training as a way to enhance an athlete’s performance in high-intensity sports. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports, 20(2), 7887. doi:10.1111/j.1600-0838.2010.01188.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Carriere J.S., Seli P., & Smilek D. (2013). Wandering in both mind and body: Individual differences in mind wandering and inattention predict fidgeting. Canadian Journal of Experimental Psychology/Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale, 67(1), 1931. PubMed ID: 26513169 doi:10.1037/a0031438

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Christoff K. (2012). Undirected thoughts: Neural determinants and correlates. Brain Research, 1428, 5159. PubMed ID: 22071565 doi:10.1016/j.brainres.2011.09.060

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Coakley J. (2004). Sports in society: Issues and controversies. Boston, MA: McGraw-Hill.

  • Connolly C., & Janelle C. (2003). Attentional strategies in rowing: Performance, perceived exertion, and gender considerations. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 15(3), 195212. doi:10.1080/10413200305387

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Englert C., Bertrams A., Furley P., & Oudejans R.R. (2015). Is ego depletion associated with increased distractibility? Results from a basketball free throw task. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 18, 2631. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2014.12.001

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Farley J., Risko E., & Kingstone A. (2013). Everyday attention and lecture retention: The effects of time, fidgeting, and mind wandering. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 619. PubMed ID: 24065933 doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00619

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fox K.C.R, Spreng N., Ellamil M., Andrews-Hanna J.R., & Christoff K. (2015). The wandering brain: Meta-analysis of functional neuroimaging studies of mind-wandering and related spontaneous thought processes. Neuroimage, 111, 611621. PubMed ID: 25725466 doi:10.1016/j.neuroimage.2015.02.039

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Franklin M.S., Mrazek M.D., Anderson C.L., Smallwood J., Kingstone A., & Schooler J. (2013). The silver lining of a mind in the clouds: Interesting musings are associated with positive mood while mind-wandering. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 583. PubMed ID: 24009599 doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00583

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Garcia S., Razon S., Hristovski R., Balaguer N., & Tenenbaum G. (2015). Dynamic stability of task-related thoughts in trained runners. The Sport Psychologist, 29, 302309. doi:10.1123/tsp.2014-0094

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Grosu E.F., Grosu V.T., Popovici C., & Preja C.A. (2014, June). The characteristics of attention in perceptual-motor disciplines: Alpine skiing and judo. Paper presented at the 4th International Congress of Physical Education, Sport and Kinetotherapy (ICSPEK), Bukarest, Romania.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Guerrero M.C.M. (2005). Inner speech-L2: thinking words in a second language. NewYork, NY: Springer.

  • Hollis R.B., & Was C.A. (2016). Mind wandering, control failures, and social media distractions in online learning. Learning and Instruction, 42, 104112. doi:10.1016/j.learninstruc.2016.01.007

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hutchinson J.C., & Tenenbaum G. (2007). Attention focus during physical effort: The mediating role of task intensity. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 8, 233245. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2006.03.006

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jones L., Karageorghis C.I., & Ekkekakis P. (2014). Can high-intensity exercise be more pleasant?: attentional dissociation using music and video. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 36(5), 528541. PubMed ID: 25356615 doi:10.1123/jsep.2013-0251

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jones L., Karageorghis C.I., Lane A.M., & Bishop D.T. (2015). The influence of motivation and attentional style on affective, cognitive, and behavioral outcomes of an exercise class. Scandinavian Journal of Medicine & Science in Sports, 27, 124135. PubMed ID: 26530034 doi:10.1111/sms.12577

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Karatekin C. (2004). A test of the integrity of the components of Baddeley’s model of working memory in attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder (ADHD). Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 45, 912926. PubMed ID: 15225335 doi:10.1111/j.1469-7610.2004.t01-1-00285.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Klinger E. (2009). Daydreaming and fantasizing: thought flow and motivation. In K.D. Markman, W.M.P. Klein, & J.A. Suhr (Eds.), Handbook of imagination and mental simulation (pp. 225239). New York, NY: Psychology Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lagarde E., Gabaude C., Maury B., Lemercier C., Salmi L.R., & Galéra C. (2012). Mind wandering and driving. Injury Prevention, 18(Suppl. 1), 200. doi:10.1136/injuryprev-2012-040590t.6

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Latinjak A.T. (2012). The underlying structure of emotions: A tri-dimensional model of core affect and emotion concepts for sports. Revista Iberoamericana de Psicología del Ejercicio y el Deporte, 7(1), 7187.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Levinson D.B., Smallwood J., & Davidson R.J. (2012). The persistence of thought: Evidence for a role of working memory in the maintenance of task-unrelated thinking. Psychological Science, 23(4), 375380. PubMed ID: 22421205 doi:10.1177/0956797611431465

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lohse K., & Sherwood D.E. (2011). Defining the focus of attention: Effects of attention on perceived exertion and fatigue. Frontiers in Psychology, 2, 332. PubMed ID: 22102843 doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2011.00332

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mason M.F., Norton M.I., Van Horn J.D., Wegner D.M., Grafton S.T., & Macrae C.N. (2007). Wandering minds: The default network and stimulus-independent thought. Science, 315(5810), 393395. PubMed ID: 17234951 doi:10.1126/science.1131295

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • McVay J.C., & Kane M.J. (2010). Does mind wandering reflect executive function or executive failure? Comment on Smallwood and Schooler (2006) and Watkins (2008). Psychological Bulletin, 136(2), 188197. PubMed ID: 20192557 doi:10.1037/a0018298

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mrazek M.D., Phillips D.T., Franklin M.S., Broadway J.M., & Schooler J.W. (2013). Young and restless: validation of the Mind-Wandering Questionnaire (MWQ) reveals disruptive impact of mind-wandering for youth. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 560. PubMed ID: 23986739 doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00560

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Nunnally J.C., & Bernstein I.H. (1994). Psychometric theory. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

  • Poerio G.L., Totterdell P., & Miles E. (2013). Mind-wandering and negative mood: Does one thing really lead to another? Consciousness and Cognition, 22, 14121421. doi:10.1016/j.concog.2013.09.012

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Seli P., Carriere J.S., Levene M., & Smilek D. (2013). How few and far between? Examining the effects of probe rate on self-reported mind wandering. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 430. PubMed ID: 23882239

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Seli P., Carriere J.S., & Smilek D. (2015). Not all mind wandering is created equal: Dissociating deliberate from spontaneous mind wandering. Psychological Research, 79(5), 750758. PubMed ID: 25284016 doi:10.1007/s00426-014-0617-x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Seli P., Wammes J.D., Risko E.F., & Smilek D. (2015). On the relation between motivation and retention in educational contexts: The role of intentional and unintentional mind wandering. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 4, 12801287 doi:10.3758/s13423-015-0979-0

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Smallwood J., O’Connor R.C., & Heim D. (2005). Rumination, dysphoria, and subjective experience. Imagination, Cognition and Personality, 24(4), 355367. doi:10.2190/AE18-AD1V-YF7L-EKBX

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Smallwood J., & Schooler J.W. (2006). The restless mind. Psychological Bulletin, 132(6), 946958. PubMed ID: 17073528 doi:10.1037/0033-2909.132.6.946

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Smallwood J., & Schooler J.W. (2015). The science of mind wandering: Empirically navigating the stream of consciousness. Annual Review of Psychology, 66, 487518. PubMed ID: 25293689 doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-010814-015331

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sparkes A.C., & Smith B. (2014). Qualitative research methods in sport, exercise and health: From process to product. London, UK: Routledge.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Stanley C.T., Pargman D., & Tenenbaum G. (2007). The effect of attentional coping strategies on perceived exertion in a cycling task. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 19, 352363. doi:10.1080/10413200701345403

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Stawarczyk D., & D’Argembeau A. (2016). Conjoint influence of mind-wandering and sleepiness on task performance. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception & Performance, 42, 15871600. PubMed ID: 27268466 doi:10.1037/xhp0000254

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Szpunar K.K., Moulton S.T., & Schacter D.L. (2013). Mind wandering and education: from the classroom to online learning. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 495. PubMed ID: 23914183 doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00495

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
All Time Past Year Past 30 Days
Abstract Views 2650 1052 29
Full Text Views 26 6 0
PDF Downloads 28 7 0