The Effect of Choice on Motor Learning for Learners With Different Levels of Intrinsic Motivation

in Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology
Restricted access

Purchase article

USD $24.95

Student 1 year subscription

USD $84.00

1 year subscription

USD $111.00

Student 2 year subscription

USD $159.00

2 year subscription

USD $208.00

This study examines whether the positive effect of choice on motor learning in a dart-throwing task varies by intrinsic motivation. Participants were allocated to a highly motivated or less-motivated group based on measured task motivation and randomly to a Choice or No Choice group. In Experiment 1, participants in the Choice group chose their dart color. In Experiment 2, they chose when to observe a model demonstration. Results showed that the effect of choice on motor learning differed between highly and less-motivated participants in Experiment 1 (i.e., interaction between motivation and choice) but not Experiment 2 (i.e., main effects of motivation and choice). Specifically, motor learning was enhanced in less-motivated but not highly motivated participants when choosing dart color, while it was enhanced regardless of initial intrinsic motivation when choosing model-demonstration time. Therefore, external provision of choice in a motor-learning situation may not be equally effective across learners.

Ikudome, Mori, and Nakamoto are with the Faculty of Physical Education, and Kou, the Graduate School of Physical Education, National Inst. of Fitness and Sports in Kanoya, Kanoya, Japan. Ogasa is with the Graduate School of Information Science and Technology, Osaka University, Osaka, Japan.

Ikudome (ra_sa1101@yahoo.co.jp) is corresponding author.
Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology

Article Sections

References

  • CarterM.J. & Ste-MarieD.M. (2016). An interpolated activity during the knowledge-of-results delay interval eliminates the learning advantages of self-controlled feedback schedules. Psychological Research 81 399406. PubMed ID: 26892773 doi:10.1007/s00426-016-0757-2

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • ChiviacowskyS. (2014). Self-controlled practice: Autonomy protects perceptions of competence and enhances motor learning. Psychology of Sport and Exercise 15(5) 505510. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2014.05.003

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • ChiviacowskyS. & WulfG. (2002). Self-controlled feedback: Does it enhance learning because performers get feedback when they need it? Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 73(4) 408415. PubMed ID: 12495242 doi:10.1080/02701367.2002.10609040

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • DeciE.L. (1971). Effects of externally mediated rewards on intrinsic motivation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 18(1) 105115. doi:10.1037/h0030644

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • DeciE.L. & RyanR.M. (1985). Intrinsic motivation and self-determination in human behavior. New York, NY: Plenum.

  • Del ReyP.WhitehurstM. & WoodJ.M. (1983). Effects of experience and contextual interference on learning and transfer by boys and girls. Perceptual and Motor Skills 56 581582. doi:10.2466/pms.1983.56.2.581

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • FaulF.ErdfelderE.LangA.G. & BuchnerA. (2007). G*power 3: A flexible statistical power analysis program for the social, behavioral, and biomedical sciences. Behavior Research Methods 39(2) 175191. PubMed ID: 17695343 doi:10.3758/BF03193146

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • GoodeS. & MagillR.A. (1986). Contextual interference effects in learning three badminton serves. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 57(4) 308314. doi:10.1080/02701367.1986.10608091

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • GrandK.F.BruziA.T.DykeF.B.GodwinM.M.LeikerA.M.ThompsonA.G.MillerM.W. (2015). Why self-controlled feedback enhances motor learning: Answers from electroencephalography and indices of motivation. Human Movement Science 432332. PubMed ID: 26163375 doi:10.1016/j.humov.2015.06.013

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • IyengarS.S. & LepperM.R. (2000). When choice is demotivating: Can one desire too much of a good thing? Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 79(6) 9951006. PubMed ID: 11138768 doi:10.1037/0022-3514.79.6.995

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • JanelleC.M.BarbaD.A.FrehlichS.G.TennantL.K. & CauraughJ.H. (1997). Maximizing performance feedback effectiveness through videotape replay and a self-controlled learning environment. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 68(4) 269279. PubMed ID: 9421839 doi:10.1080/02701367.1997.10608008

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • KantakS.S.SullivanK.J.FisherB.E.KnowltonB.J. & WinsteinC.J. (2010). Neural substrates of motor memory consolidation depend on practice structure. Nature Neuroscience 13(8) 923925. PubMed ID: 20622872 doi:10.1038/nn.2596

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • KantakS.S.SullivanK.J.FisherB.E.KnowltonB.J. & WinsteinC.J. (2011). Transfer of motor learning engages specific neural substrates during motor memory consolidation dependent on the practice structure. Journal of Motor Behavior 43(6) 499507. PubMed ID: 22150021 doi:10.1080/00222895.2011.632657

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • LeeT.D.SwinnenS.P. & SerrienD. (1994). Cognitive effort and motor learning. Quest 46(3) 328344. doi:10.1080/00336297.1994.10484130

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • LemosA.WulfG.LewthwaiteR. & ChiviacowskyS. (2017). Autonomy support enhances performance expectancies, positive affect, and motor learning. Psychology of Sport and Exercise 31 2834. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2017.03.009

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • LeottiL.A. & DelgadoM.R. (2014). The value of exercising control over monetary gains and losses. Psychological Science 25(2) 596604. PubMed ID: 24390827 doi:10.1177/0956797613514589

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • LewthwaiteR.ChiviacowskyS.DrewsR. & WulfG. (2015). Choose to move: The motivational impact of autonomy support on motor learning. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 22(5) 13831388. PubMed ID: 25732095 doi:10.3758/s13423-015-0814-7

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MurayamaK.MatsumotoM.IzumaK.SugiuraA.RyanR.M.DeciE.L. & MatsumotoK. (2015). How self-determined choice facilitates performance: A key role of the ventromedial prefrontal cortex. Cerebral Cortex 25(5) 12411251. PubMed ID: 24297329 doi:10.1093/cercor/bht317

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • PatallE.A. (2012). The motivational complexity of choosing: A review of theory and research. In R.M. Ryan (Ed.) The Oxford handbook of human motivation (pp. 248279). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • PatallE.A. (2013). Constructing motivation through choice, interest, and interestingness. Journal of Educational Psychology 105(2) 522534. doi:10.1037/a0030307

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • PatallE.A.CooperH. & RobinsonJ.C. (2008). The effects of choice on intrinsic motivation and related outcomes: A meta-analysis of research findings. Psychological Bulletin 134(2) 270300. PubMed ID: 18298272 doi:10.1037/0033-2909.134.2.270

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • PollockB.J. & LeeT.D. (1997). Dissociated contextual interference effects in children and adults. Perceptual and Motor Skills 84 851858. PubMed ID: 9172193 doi:10.2466/pms.1997.84.3.851

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • RyanR.M. (1982). Control and information in the intrapersonal sphere: An extension of cognitive evaluation theory. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 43(3) 450461. doi:10.1037/0022-3514.43.3.450

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • RyanR.M. & DeciE.L. (2017). Self-determination theory: Basic psychological needs in motivation development and wellness. New York, NY: Guilford.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SanliE.A.PattersonJ.T.BrayS.R. & LeeT.D. (2012). Understanding self-controlled motor learning protocols through the self-determination theory. Frontiers in Psychology 3 117. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2012.00611

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SheaC.H. & KohlR.M. (1990). Specificity and variability of practice. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 61(2) 169177. PubMed ID: 2094928 doi:10.1080/02701367.1990.10608671

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SheaC.H. & KohlR.M. (1991). Composition of practice: Influence on the retention of motor skills. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 62(2) 187195. PubMed ID: 1925042 doi:10.1080/02701367.1991.10608709

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • SheaJ.B. & MorganR.L. (1979). Contextual interference effects on the acquisition, retention, and transfer of a motor skill. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory 5(3) 179187. doi:10.1037/0278-7393.5.2.179

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Sullivan-TooleH.RicheyJ.A. & TricomiE. (2017). Control and effort costs influence the motivational consequences of choice. Frontiers in Psychology 8115. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2017.00675

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WrisbergG.A. & PeinR.L. (2002). Note on learners’ control of the frequency of model presentation during skill acquisition. Perceptual and Motor Skills 94 792794. PubMed ID: 12081283 doi:10.2466/PMS.94.2.792-794

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WulfG. & AdamsN. (2014). Small choices can enhance balance learning. Human Movement Science 38235240. PubMed ID: 25457421 doi:10.1016/j.humov.2014.10.007

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WulfG.ChiviacowskyS. & CardozoP.L. (2014). Additive benefits of autonomy support and enhanced expectancies for motor learning. Human Movement Science 37 1220. PubMed ID: 25046402 doi:10.1016/j.humov.2014.06.004

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WulfG.ClaussA.SheaC.H. & WhitacreC.A. (2001). Benefits of self-control in dyad practice. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 72(3) 299303. PubMed ID: 11561396 doi:10.1080/02701367.2001.10608964

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WulfG.RaupachM. & PfeifferF. (2005). Self-controlled observational practice enhances learning. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 76(1) 107111. PubMed ID: 15810775 doi:10.1080/02701367.2005.10599266

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation

Article Metrics

All Time Past Year Past 30 Days
Abstract Views 172 172 130
Full Text Views 25 25 23
PDF Downloads 8 8 7

Altmetric Badge

PubMed

Google Scholar