Attention, Perception, and Action in a Simulated Decision-Making Task

Click name to view affiliation

Stefanie Hüttermann German Sport University Cologne

Search for other papers by Stefanie Hüttermann in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
*
,
Paul R. Ford University of Brighton

Search for other papers by Paul R. Ford in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
*
,
A. Mark Williams The University of Utah

Search for other papers by A. Mark Williams in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
*
,
Matyas Varga University of Brighton

Search for other papers by Matyas Varga in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
*
, and
Nicholas J. Smeeton University of Brighton

Search for other papers by Nicholas J. Smeeton in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
*
Restricted access

Over the last decade, research on the visual focus of attention has become increasingly popular in psychological science. The focus of attention has been shown to be important in fast team-sport games. The authors developed a method that measures the extent of the attentional focus and perceptual capabilities during performance of a sport-specific task. The participants were required to judge different player configurations on their left and right sides with varying visual angles between the stimuli. In keeping with the notion that the focus of attention is smaller than the visual field, attentional performance was poorest at the wider viewing angles compared with perceptual performance. Moreover, the team-sport players were better able to enlarge their attentional focus and make correct decisions more frequently than individual athletes, particularly when a motor response was required. The findings provide a new perspective, dissociating the attentional and perceptual processes that affect decision making under various response modes.

Hüttermann is with the Dept. of Cognitive and Team/Racket Sport Research, Inst. of Exercise Training and Sport Informatics, German Sport University Cologne, Cologne, Germany. Ford, Varga, and Smeeton are with Welkin Laboratories, School of Sport and Service Management, University of Brighton, Eastbourne, United Kingdom. Williams is with the Dept. of Health, Kinesiology, and Recreation, College of Health, University of Utah, Salt Lake City, UT.

Hüttermann (s.huettermann@dshs-koeln.de) is corresponding author.
  • Collapse
  • Expand
  • Casanova, F., Garganta, J., Silva, G., Alves, A., Oliveira, J., & Williams, A.M. (2013). Effects of prolonged intermittent exercise on perceptual-cognitive processes. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise, 45, 16101617. PubMed ID: 23470295 doi:10.1249/MSS.0b013e31828b2ce9

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Choi, B., Lee, S., Lee, J.E., Hong, S., Lee, J., & Kim, S. (2015). A study on the optimum curvature for the curved monitor. Journal of Information Display, 16, 217223. doi:10.1080/15980316.2015.1111847

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dehaene, S., Changeaux, J.P., Naccache, L., Sackur, J., & Sergent, C. (2006). Conscious, preconscious, and subliminal processing: A testable taxonomy. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 10, 204211. PubMed ID: 16603406 doi:10.1016/j.tics.2006.03.007

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ericsson, K.A., & Lehmann, A.C. (1996). Expert and exceptional performance: Evidence of maximal adaptation to task. Annual Review of Psychology, 47, 273305. PubMed ID: 15012483 doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.47.1.273

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Eysenck, M.W., & Calvo, M.G. (1992). Anxiety and performance: The processing efficiency theory. Cognition and Emotion, 6, 409434. doi:10.1080/02699939208409696

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Farrow, D., & Abernethy, B. (2003). Do expertise and the degree of perception—Action coupling affect natural anticipatory performance? Perception, 32, 11271139. PubMed ID: 14651325 doi:10.1068/p3323

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Goodale, M.A., & Milner, A.D. (1992). Separate visual pathways for perception and action. Trends in Neurosciences, 15, 2025. doi:10.1016/0166-2236(92)90344-8

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hüttermann, S., Bock, O., & Memmert, D. (2012). The breadth of attention in old age. Ageing Research, 10, 6770.

  • Hüttermann, S., Helsen, W.F., Put, K., & Memmert, D. (2018). Does visual attention impact on decision making in complex dynamic events? Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 40, 163166. doi:10.1123/jsep.2017-0169

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hüttermann, S., & Memmert, D. (2014). Does the inverted-U function disappear in expert athletes? An analysis of the attentional behavior under physical exercise of athletes and non-athletes. Physiology & Behavior, 131, 8792. doi:10.1016/j.physbeh.2014.04.020

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hüttermann, S., & Memmert, D. (2015). The influence of motivational and mood states on visual attention: A quantification of systematic differences and casual changes in subjects’ focus of attention. Cognition and Emotion, 29, 471483. doi:10.1080/02699931.2014.920767

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hüttermann, S., & Memmert, D. (2017). The attention window: A narrative review of limitations and opportunities influencing the focus of attention. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport, 88, 169183. doi:10.1080/02701367.2017.1293228

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hüttermann, S., & Memmert, D. (2018). Effects of lab- and field-based attentional training on athletes’ attention-window. Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 38, 1727. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2018.05.009

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hüttermann, S., Memmert, D., & Simons, D.J. (2014). The size and shape of the attentional “spotlight” varies with differences in sports expertise. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 20, 147157.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hüttermann, S., Memmert, D., Simons, D.J., & Bock, O. (2013). Fixation strategy influences the ability to focus attention on two spatially separate objects. PLoS ONE, 8, e65673.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hüttermann, S., Smeeton, N.J., Ford, P.R., & Williams, A.M. (2019). Color perception and attentional load in dynamic, time-constrained environments. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 2614. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.02614

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Intriligator, J., & Cavanagh, P. (2001). The spatial resolution of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 43, 171216. PubMed ID: 11689021 doi:10.1006/cogp.2001.0755

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mann, D.L., Abernethy, B., & Farrow, D. (2010). Visual information underpinning skilled anticipation: The effect of blur on a coupled and uncoupled in situ anticipatory response. Attention, Perception & Psychophysics, 72, 13171326. doi:10.3758/APP.72.5.1317

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mann, D.T., Williams, A.M., Ward, P., & Janelle, C.M. (2007). Perceptual cognitive expertise in sport: A meta-analysis. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology, 29, 457478. doi:10.1123/jsep.29.4.457

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Milner, A.D., & Goodale, M.A. (1995). The visual brain in action. Oxford, UK: Oxford Press.

  • Müller, S., & Rosalie, S.M. (2019). Transfer of expert visual-perceptual-motor skill in sport. In R. Jackson& A.M. Williams. Perception and decision making in sport: Theories and applications (pp. 375393). Abingdon, UK: Routledge.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Raab, M. (2003). Decision making in sports: Influence of complexity on implicit and explicit learning. International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 1, 406433. doi:10.1080/1612197X.2003.9671728

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Redick, T.S., Broadway, J.M., Meier, M.E., Kuriakose, P.S., Unsworth, N., Kane, M.J., & Engle, R.W. (2012). Measuring working memory capacity with automated complex span tasks. European Journal of Psychological Assessment, 28, 164171. doi:10.1027/1015-5759/a000123

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Roca, A., & Williams, A.M. (2017). Does decision making transfer across similar and dissimilar sports? Psychology of Sport and Exercise, 31, 4043. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2017.04.004

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schneider, W., Dumais, S.T., & Shiffrin, R.M. (1984). Automatic and control processing and attention. In R. Parasuraman & D.R. Davies, Eds. Varieties of attention (pp. 127). Orlando, FL: Academic Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Smeeton, N.J., Ward, P., & Williams, A.M. (2004). Do pattern recognition skills transfer across sports? A preliminary analysis. Journal of Sports Sciences, 22(2),205213. PubMed ID: 14998098 doi:10.1080/02640410310001641494

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tenenbaum, G., & Bar-Eli, M. (1993). Decision making in sport: A cognitive perspective. In R.N. Singer, M. Murphey, & L.K. Tennant (Eds.), Handbook of research on sport psychology (pp. 171192). New York, NY: Macmillan.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Treisman, A., & Gelade, G. (1980). A feature-integration theory of attention. Cognitive Psychology, 12, 97136. PubMed ID: 7351125 doi:10.1016/0010-0285(80)90005-5

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Unsworth, N., Heitz, R.P., Schrock, J.C., & Engle, R.W. (2005). An automated version of the operation span task. Behavior Research Method, 37, 498505. doi:10.3758/BF03192720

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • van der Kamp, J., Rivas, F., van Doorn, H., & Savelsbergh, G. (2008). Ventral and dorsal system contributions to visual anticipation in fast ball sports. International Journal of Sport Psychology, 39, 100130.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Vater, C., Roca, A., & Williams, A.M. (2016). Effects of anxiety on anticipation and visual search in dynamic, time-constrained situations. Sport, Exercise, and Performance Psychology, 5, 179192. doi:10.1037/spy0000056

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Voss, M.W., Kramer, A.F., Basak, C., Prakash, R.S., & Roberts, B. (2010). Are expert athletes “expert” in the cognitive laboratory? A meta-analytic study of cognition and sport expertise. Applied Cognitive Psychology, 24, 812826. doi:10.1002/acp.1588

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Williams, A.M., Davids, K., & Williams, J.G. (1999). Visual perception and action in sport. London, UK: E & F.N. Spon.

  • Williams, A.M., Ward, P., Smeeton, N.J., & Allen, D. (2004). Developing anticipation skills in tennis using on-court instruction: Perception versus perception and action. Journal of Applied Sport Psychology, 16(4),350360. doi:10.1080/10413200490518002

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wolfe, B., Dobres, J., Rosenholtz, R., & Reimer, B. (2017). More than the useful field: Considering peripheral vision in driving. Applied Ergonomics, 65, 316325. PubMed ID: 28802451 doi:10.1016/j.apergo.2017.07.009

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
All Time Past Year Past 30 Days
Abstract Views 5239 880 63
Full Text Views 389 81 9
PDF Downloads 945 72 8