The Influence of Affective Priming on the Affective Response During Exercise: A Replication Study

Click name to view affiliation

Sinika Timme Sport and Exercise Psychology, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany

Search for other papers by Sinika Timme in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Jasmin Hutchinson Exercise Science and Athletic Training, Springfield College, Springfield, MA, USA

Search for other papers by Jasmin Hutchinson in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Anton Regorius Sport and Exercise Psychology, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany

Search for other papers by Anton Regorius in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
, and
Ralf Brand Sport and Exercise Psychology, University of Potsdam, Potsdam, Germany

Search for other papers by Ralf Brand in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
*
Restricted access

The affective response during exercise is an important factor for long-term exercise adherence. Pottratz et al. suggested affective priming as a behavioral intervention for the enhancement of exercise-related affect. The present paper aims to replicate and extend upon these findings. We conducted a close replication with 53 participants completing a brisk walking task in two conditions (prime vs. no prime). Affective valence was assessed during exercise, and exercise enjoyment and remembered/forecasted pleasure were assessed postexercise. We could not replicate the findings of Pottratz et al., finding no evidence for positive changes in psychological responses in the priming condition. However, linear mixed models demonstrated significant interindividual differences in how participants responded to priming. These results demonstrate that affective priming during exercise does not work for everyone under every circumstance and, thus, provide an important contribution to the understanding of boundary conditions and moderating factors for priming in exercise psychology.

  • Collapse
  • Expand
  • Addoh, O., Sanders, R., & Loprinzi, P. (2020). Experimental effects of priming on affective responses to acute exercise. Psych, 2(1), 5473. https://doi.org/10.3390/psych2010007

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Antoniewicz, F., & Brand, R. (2014). Automatic evaluations and exercise setting preference in frequent exercisers. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 36(6), 631636. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.2014-0033

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Antoniewicz, F., & Brand, R. (2016). Dropping out or keeping up? Early-dropouts, late-dropouts, and maintainers differ in their automatic evaluations of exercise already before a 14-week exercise course. Frontiers in Psychology, 7, Article 838. https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2016.00838

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Aronson, E., Wilson, T.D., Akert, R.M., & Sommers, S.R. (2016). Social psychology (9th ed.). Person.

  • Banks, G.C., Rogelberg, S.G., Woznyj, H.M., Landis, R.S., & Rupp, D.E. (2016). Evidence on questionable research practices: The good, the bad, and the ugly. Journal of Business and Psychology, 31(3), 323338. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10869-016-9456-7

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Banting, L.K., Dimmock, J.A., & Grove, J.R. (2011). The impact of automatically activated motivation on exercise-related outcomes. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 33(4), 569585. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.33.4.569

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bargh, J.A., & Chartrand, T.L. (2014). The mind in the middle: A practical guide to priming and automaticity research. In H.T. Reis & C.M. Judd (Eds.), Handbook of research methods in social and personality psychology (pp. 311344). Cambridge University Press.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bargh, J.A., Chen, M., & Burrows, L. (1996). Automaticity of social behavior: Direct effects of trait construct and stereotype activation on action. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 71(2), 230244. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.71.2.230

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bargh, J.A., & Pietromonaco, P. (1982). Automatic information processing and social perception: The influence of trait information presented outside of conscious awareness on impression formation. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 43(3), 437449. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.43.3.437

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Blanchfield, A., Hardy, J., & Marcora, S. (2014). Non-conscious visual cues related to affect and action alter perception of effort and endurance performance. Frontiers in Human Neuroscience, 8, Article 967. https://doi.org/10.3389/fnhum.2014.00967

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Borg, G. (1998). Borg’s perceived exertion and pain scales. Human kinetics.

  • Brand, R., & Ekkekakis, P. (2018). Affective–reflective theory of physical inactivity and exercise. German Journal of Exercise and Sport Research, 48(1), 4858. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12662-017-0477-9

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Brandt, M.J., IJzerman, H., Dijksterhuis, A., Farach, F.J., Geller, J., Giner-Sorolla, R., Grange, J.A., Perugini, M., Spies, J.R., & van ’t Veer, A. (2014). The replication recipe: What makes for a convincing replication? Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 50, 217224. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jesp.2013.10.005

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cesario, J. (2014). Priming, replication, and the hardest science. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9(1), 4048. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691613513470

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cesario, J., & Jonas, K.J. (2014). Replicability and models of priming: What a resource computation framework can tell us about expectations of replicability. Social Cognition, 32(Suppl. 1), 124136. https://doi.org/10.1521/soco.2014.32.supp.124

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chen, M.J., Fan, X., & Moe, S.T. (2002). Criterion-related validity of the Borg ratings of perceived exertion scale in healthy individuals: A meta-analysis. Journal of Sports Sciences, 20(11), 873899. https://doi.org/10.1080/026404102320761787

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Collins, A.M., & Loftus, E.F. (1975). A spreading-activation theory of semantic processing. Psychological Review, 82, 407428. https://doi.org/10.1037/0033-295X.82.6.407

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Debner, J.A., & Jacoby, L.L. (1994). Unconscious perception: Attention, awareness, and control. Journal of Experimental Psychology. Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 20(2), 304317. https://doi.org/10.1037//0278-7393.20.2.304

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dijksterhuis, A., Aarts, H., Bargh, J.A., & van Knippenberg, A. (2000). On the relation between associative strength and automatic behavior. Journal of Experimental Social Psychology, 36(5), 531544. https://doi.org/10.1006/jesp.2000.1427

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Donges, U.-S., Kersting, A., & Suslow, T. (2012). Women’s greater ability to perceive happy facial emotion automatically: Gender differences in affective priming. PLoS One, 7(7), Article e41745. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0041745

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Doyen, S., Klein, O., Pichon, C.-L., & Cleeremans, A. (2012). Behavioral priming: It’s all in the mind, but whose mind? PLoS One, 7(1), Article e29081. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0029081

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Ekkekakis, P., Parfitt, G., & Petruzzello, S.J. (2011). The pleasure and displeasure people feel when they exercise at different intensities. Sports Medicine, 41(8), 641671. https://doi.org/10.2165/11590680-000000000-00000

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fazio, R.H. (2001). On the automatic activation of associated evaluations: An overview. Cognition & Emotion, 15(2), 115141. https://doi.org/10.1080/0269993004200024

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fazio, R.H., Sanbonmatsu, D.M., Powell, M.C., & Kardes, F.R. (1986). On the automatic activation of evaluations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 50, 229238. https://doi.org/10.1037/0022-3514.50.2.229

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Foster, P.S., Hubbard, T., Campbell, R.W., Poole, J., Pridmore, M., Bell, C., & Harrison, D.W. (2017). Spreading activation in emotional memory networks and the cumulative effects of somatic markers. Brain Informatics, 4(2), 8593. https://doi.org/10.1007/s40708-016-0054-2

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Fredriksson, S.V., Alley, S.J., Rebar, A.L., Hayman, M., Vandelanotte, C., & Schoeppe, S. (2018). How are different levels of knowledge about physical activity associated with physical activity behaviour in Australian adults? PLoS One, 13(11), Article e0207003. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0207003

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gendolla, G.H.E., & Silvestrini, N. (2011). Smiles make it easier and so do frowns: Masked affective stimuli influence mental effort. Emotion, 11(2), 320328. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0022593

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hardy, C.J., & Rejeski, W.J. (1989). Not what, but how one feels: The measurement of affect during exercise. Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology, 11(3), 304317. https://doi.org/10.1123/jsep.11.3.304

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hutchinson, J.C., Jones, L., Vitti, S.N., Moore, A., Dalton, P.C., & O’Neil, B.J. (2018). The influence of self-selected music on affect-regulated exercise intensity and remembered pleasure during treadmill running. Sport, Exercise, and Performance Psychology, 7(1), 8092. https://doi.org/10.1037/spy0000115

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jekauc, D., Voelkle, M., Wagner, M.O., Mewes, N., & Woll, A. (2013). Reliability, validity, and measurement invariance of the German version of the physical activity enjoyment scale. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 38(1), 104115. https://doi.org/10.1093/jpepsy/jss088

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • John, L.K., Loewenstein, G., & Prelec, D. (2012). Measuring the prevalence of questionable research practices with incentives for truth telling. Psychological Science, 23(5), 524532. https://doi.org/10.1177/0956797611430953

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jones, L., & Zenko, Z. (2021). Strategies to facilitate more pleasant exercise experiences. In Z. Zenko & L. Jones (Eds.), Essentials of exercise and sport psychology: An open access textbook (pp. 242270). Society for Transparency, Openness, and Replication in Kinesiology. https://doi.org/10.51224/B1011

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Karageorghis, C.I., & Terry, P.C. (1997). The psychophysical effects of music in sport and exercise: A review. Journal of Sport Behavior, 20(1), 54.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lakens, D. (2012). Polarity correspondence in metaphor congruency effects: Structural overlap predicts categorization times for bipolar concepts presented in vertical space. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 38(3), 726736. https://doi.org/10.1037/a0024955

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lishner, D.A., Cooter, A.B., & Zald, D.H. (2008). Addressing measurement limitations in affective rating scales: Development of an empirical valence scale. Cognition and Emotion, 22(1), 180192. https://doi.org/10.1080/02699930701319139

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Maas, C.J.M., & Hox, J.J. (2004). Robustness issues in multilevel regression analysis. Statistica Neerlandica, 58(2), 127137. https://doi.org/10.1046/j.0039-0402.2003.00252.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Matuschek, H., Kliegl, R., Vasishth, S., Baayen, H., & Bates, D. (2017). Balancing Type I error and power in linear mixed models. Journal of Memory and Language, 94, 305315. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jml.2017.01.001

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mullen, S.P., Olson, E.A., Phillips, S.M., Szabo, A.N., Wójcicki, T.R., Mailey, E.L., Gothe, N.P., Fanning, J.T., Kramer, A.F., & McAuley, E. (2011). Measuring enjoyment of physical activity in older adults: Invariance of the physical activity enjoyment scale (paces) across groups and time. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 8(1), Article 103. https://doi.org/10.1186/1479-5868-8-103

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Noble, B.J., & Robertson, R.J. (1996). Perceived exertion. Human Kinetics.

  • Open Science Collaboration. (2015). Estimating the reproducibility of psychological science. Science, 349(6251), Article aac4716. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.aac4716

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pashler, H., Coburn, N., & Harris, C.R. (2012). Priming of social distance? Failure to replicate effects on social and food judgments. PLoS One, 7(8), Article e42510. https://doi.org/10.1371/journal.pone.0042510

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Piercy, K.L., Troiano, R.P., Ballard, R.M., Carlson, S.A., Fulton, J.E., Galuska, D.A., . . . Olson, R.D. (2018). The physical activity guidelines for Americans. JAMA, 320(19), 20202028. https://doi.org/10.1001/jama.2018.14854

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pottratz, S.T., Hutchinson, J.C., Karageorghis, C.I., Mullin, E.M., & Zenko, Z. (2021). Prime movers: Effects of subliminal primes, music, and music video on psychological responses to exercise. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 55(2), 112122. https://doi.org/10.1093/abm/kaaa036

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rebar, A.L., Dimmock, J.A., Jackson, B., Rhodes, R.E., Kates, A., Starling, J., & Vandelanotte, C. (2016). A systematic review of the effects of non-conscious regulatory processes in physical activity. Health Psychology Review, 10(4), 395407. https://doi.org/10.1080/17437199.2016.1183505

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rhodes, R.E., & Kates, A. (2015). Can the affective response to exercise predict future motives and physical activity behavior? A systematic review of published evidence. Annals of Behavioral Medicine, 49(5), 715731. https://doi.org/10.1007/s12160-015-9704-5

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Riebe, D., Ehrman, J.K., Liguori, G., Magal, M., & American College of Sports Medicine (Eds.). (2018). ACSM’s guidelines for exercise testing and prescription. Wolters Kluwer.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Simons, D.J. (2014). The value of direct replication. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 9(1), 7680. https://doi.org/10.1177/1745691613514755

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tudor-Locke, C., Brashear, M.M., Johnson, W.D., & Katzmarzyk, P.T. (2010). Accelerometer profiles of physical activity and inactivity in normal weight, overweight, and obese U.S. men and women. International Journal of Behavioral Nutrition and Physical Activity, 7(1), 60. https://doi.org/10.1186/1479-5868-7-60

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Vohs, K.D., Mead, N.L., & Goode, M.R. (2006). The psychological consequences of money. Science, 314(5802), 11541156. https://doi.org/10.1126/science.1132491

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • World Health Organization. (2009). Global health risks: Mortality and burden of disease attributable to selected major risks.

  • Winkielman, P., Berridge, K.C., & Wilbarger, J.L. (2005). Experimental effects of priming on affective responses to acute exercise. Personality and Social Psychology Bulletin, 31, 121135. https://doi.org/10.1177/0146167204271309

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • World Health Organization. (2021, September 20). Physical activity. https://www.who.int/news-room/fact-sheets/detail/physical-activity

All Time Past Year Past 30 Days
Abstract Views 2065 822 87
Full Text Views 350 238 10
PDF Downloads 266 109 4