Ecological Physics and the Perceptual Information That Supports Motor Control

Click name to view affiliation

Thomas A. Stoffregen
Search for other papers by Thomas A. Stoffregen in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
Restricted access

Ordinary behavior, such as walking, reading, and throwing, depends on real-time perceptual guidance. In this article, I discuss the nature of perceptual information that, in principle, might be sufficient for the guidance of movement to achieve behavioral goals. I argue that we achieve behavioral goals by controlling movements relative to multiple physical referents. Movement relative to different physical referents causes changes in the structure of different forms of ambient energy (e.g., light, sound) and, therefore, to changes in sensory stimulation. I claim that movement always is controlled simultaneously relative to multiple referents, such that no single form of ambient energy can, in principle, contain information that is sufficient for successful control. The needed perceptual information exists, I claim, solely in the global array, that is, in emergent, higher-order patterns that extend across different forms of ambient energy. I review formal and empirical examples, and discuss implications for kinesiology.

Stoffregen is with the School of Kinesiology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, MN.

Address author correspondence to Thomas A. Stoffregen at tas@umn.edu.
  • Collapse
  • Expand
  • Barsalou, L.W. (2008). Grounded cognition. Annual Review of Psychology, 59, 617645. PubMed doi:10.1146/annurev.psych.59.103006.093639

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bingham, G.P. & Stassen, M.G. (1994). Monocular egocentric distance information generated by head movement. Ecological Psychology, 6, 219238. doi:10.1207/s15326969eco0603_4

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Clark, A. (1999). An embodied cognitive science? Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 3, 345351. PubMed doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01361-3

  • Clark, A. (2008). Supersizing the mind: Embodiment, action, and cognitive extension. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

  • Darwin, C. (1859). On the origin of species by means of natural selection, or the preservation of favoured races in the struggle for life. New York, NY: D. Appleton and Company.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gibson, J.J. (1966). The senses considered as perceptual systems. Boston, MA: Houghton-Mifflin.

  • Gibson, J.J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.

  • Gibson, J.J. & Gibson, E.J. (1955). Perceptual learning: Differentiation or enrichment? Psychological Review, 62, 3241. PubMed doi:10.1037/h0048826

  • Gibson, J.J., Olum, P., & Rosenblatt, F. (1955). Parallax and perspective during aircraft landings. The American Journal of Psychology, 68, 372385. PubMed doi:10.2307/1418521

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Heine, W.-D., & Guski, R. (1991). Listening: The perception of auditory events?. Ecological Psychology, 3, 263275.

  • Jones, R.K., & Lee, D.N. (1981). Why two eyes are better than one: Two view of binocular vision. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 7, 3040.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lee, D.N. (1990). Getting around with light or sound. In R. Warren & A. H. Wertheim (Eds.), Perception and control of self-motion (pp. 487506). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mantel, B., Bardy, B.G., & Stoffregen, T.A. (2010). Locomotor assessment of whether an object is reachable. Ecological Psychology, 22, 192211. doi:10.1080/10407413.2010.496665

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mantel, B., Stoffregen, T.A., Campbell, A., & Bardy, B.G. (2015). Exploratory movement generates higher-order information that is sufficient for accurate perception of scaled egocentric distance. PLoS ONE, 10(4), 0120025. PubMed doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0120025

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Matthis, J.S., & Fajen, B.R. (2014). Visual control of foot placement when walking over complex terrain. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 40, 106115.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Oman, C.M. (1982). A heuristic mathematical model for the dynamics of sensory conflict and motion sickness. Acta Otolaryngologica Supplement, 392, 144.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Oudejans, R.R.D., Michaels, C.F., Bakker, F.C., & Dolne, M.A. (1996). The relevance of action in perceiving affordances: Perception of catchableness of fly balls. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 22, 879891. PubMed

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Peper, L., Bootsma, R.J., Mestre, D.R., & Bakker, F.C. (1994). Catching balls: How to get the hand to the right place at the right time. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 20, 591612. PubMed

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pfeifer, R., & Bognard, J.C. (2006). How the body shapes the way we think. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  • Proffitt, D.R. (2006). Embodied perception and the economy of action. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 1, 110122. PubMed doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2006.00008.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Regan, D., & Gray, R. (2000). Visually guided collision avoidance and collision achievement. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 4, 99107. PubMed doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(99)01442-4

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Reed, E.S. (1996). Encountering the world: Toward an ecological psychology. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

  • Richardson, M.J., Shockley, K., Fajen, B.R., Riley, M.A., & Turvey, M.T. (2008). Ecological psychology: Six principles for an embodied-embedded approach to behavior. In P. Calvo & T. Gomila (Eds.), Handbook of cognitive science: An embodied approach (pp. 161187). Amsterdam, The Netherlands: Elsevier.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Riley, M.A., Wagman, J.B., Santana, M.-V., Carello, C., & Turvey, M.T. (2002). Perceptual behavior: Recurrence analysis of a haptic exploratory procedure. Perception, 31, 481510. PubMed doi:10.1068/p3176

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schmidt, R.A. (1982). More on motor programs. In J.A.S. Kelso (Ed.), Human motor behavior: An introduction (pp. 189217). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Shaw, R., Turvey, M.T., & Mace, W. (1982). Ecological psychology: The consequence of a commitment to realism. In W. Weimerd & D. Palermo (Eds.), Cognition and the symbolic process (pp. 159226). Hillsdale, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Inc.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Soechting, J.F., & Flanders, M. (1992). Moving in three-dimensional space: Frames of reference, vectors, and coordinate systems. Annual Review of Neuroscience, 15, 167191. PubMed doi:10.1146/annurev.ne.15.030192.001123

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Stein, B.E. & Meredith, M.A. (1993). The merging of the senses. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  • Stoffregen, T.A., & Bardy, B.G. (2001). On specification and the senses. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24, 195213. PubMed doi:10.1017/S0140525X01003946

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Stoffregen, T.A., Mantel, B., & Bardy, B.G. (2017). The senses considered as one perceptual system. Ecological Psychology, 29, 165197. doi:10.1080/10407413.2017.1331116

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Stoffregen, T.A., & Pittenger, J.B. (1995). Human echolocation as a basic form of perception and action. Ecological Psychology, 7, 181216. doi:10.1207/s15326969eco0703_2

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Stoffregen, T.A., & Riccio, G.E. (1988). An ecological theory of orientation and the vestibular system. Psychological Review, 95, 314. PubMed doi:10.1037/0033-295X.95.1.3

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Streit, M, Shockley, K, & Riley, MA. (2007). Rotational inertia and multimodal heaviness perception. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review, 14, 10011006. PubMed doi:10.3758/BF03194135

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Turvey, M.T. (1990). Coordination. American Psychologist, 45, 938953. PubMed doi:10.1037/0003-066X.45.8.938

  • Turvey, M.T., Shaw, R.E., Reed, E.S., & Mace, W.M. (1981). Ecological laws of perceiving and acting: In reply to Fodor and Pylyshyn (1981). Cognition, 9, 237304. PubMed doi:10.1016/0010-0277(81)90002-0

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Varela, F.J., Thompson, E.T., & Rosch, E. (1992). The embodied mind. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  • Von Helmholtz, H. (1962). Treatise on physiological optics (J. P. Southall, Trans.). Mineola, NY: Dover. (Original edition published in 1924.)

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
All Time Past Year Past 30 Days
Abstract Views 2508 345 134
Full Text Views 63 57 0
PDF Downloads 12 3 0