The function of the conscious field remains mysterious from a scientific point of view. This article reviews theoretical approaches (passive frame theory and ideomotor approaches) that elucidate how the conscious field is intimately related to a special kind of action selection. This form of action selection is peculiar to the skeletal-muscle output system. The notion of encapsulation and how it explains many properties of the conscious field are discussed, including why the conscious field, though in the service of adaptive action, contains contents that are not action-relevant; why the field has a first-person perspective; and why the field is so thorough, in terms of its contents, the contrasts among contents, and the representation of spatial layout. The authors discuss subordinate encapsulation and the hypothesis that the conscious field is what allows for encapsulated conscious contents to influence action selection collectively, yielding what in everyday life is called voluntary behavior.
The authors are with the Dept. of Psychology, San Francisco State University, San Francisco, CA, USA. Morsella is also with the Dept. of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, USA.
Ach, N. (1905/1951). Determining tendencies: Awareness. In D. Rapaport (Ed.), Organization and pathology of thought (pp. 15–38), New York, NY: Columbia University Press.
Agre, P.E., & Chapman, D. (1990). What are plans for? Robotics and Autonomous Systems, 6(1–2), 17–34. doi:10.1016/S0921-8890(05)80026-0
Allen, A.K., Krisst, L., Montemayor, C., & Morsella, E. (2016). Entry of involuntary conscious contents from ambiguous images. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 3(4), 326–337. doi:10.1037/cns0000095
Allen, A.K., Wilkins, K., Gazzaley, A., & Morsella, E. (2013). Conscious thoughts from reflex-like processes: A new experimental paradigm for consciousness research. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(4), 1318–1331. PubMed ID: 24056177 doi:10.1016/j.concog.2013.08.007
Anderson, M.C., Ochsner, K.N., Kuhl, B., Cooper, J., Robertson, E., Gabrieli, S.W., . . . Gabrieli, J.D.E. (2004). Neural systems underlying the suppression of unwanted memories. Science, 303(5655), 232–235. PubMed ID: 14716015 doi:10.1126/science.1089504
Arkin, R.C. (1998). Behavior-based robotics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Baars, B.J. (1988). A cognitive theory of consciousness. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.
Badets, A., & Osiurak, F. (2015). A goal-based mechanism for delayed motor intention: Considerations from motor skills, tool use and action memory. Psychological Research, 79(3), 345–360. PubMed ID: 24913016 doi:10.1007/s00426-014-0581-5
Badre, D. (2008). Cognitive control, hierarchy, and the rostro-caudal organization of the frontal lobes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(5), 193–200. PubMed ID: 18403252 doi:10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.004
Badre, D., & D’Esposito, M. (2007). Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for a hierarchical organization of the prefrontal cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(12), 2082–2099. PubMed ID: 17892391 doi:10.1162/jocn.2007.19.12.2082
Bahlmann, J., Blumenfeld, R.S., & D’Esposito, M. (2015). The rostro-caudal axis of frontal cortex is sensitive to the domain of stimulus information. Cerebral Cortex, 25(7), 1815–1826. PubMed ID: 24451658 doi:10.1093/cercor/bht419
Banks, W.P. (1995). Evidence for consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 4(2), 270–272. PubMed ID: 8521269 doi:10.1006/ccog.1995.1037
Bargh, J.A., & Morsella, E. (2008). The unconscious mind. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(1), 73–79. PubMed ID: 18584056 doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2008.00064.x
Baumeister, R.F., Vohs, K.D., DeWall, N., & Zhang, L. (2007). How emotion shapes behavior: Feedback, anticipation, and reflection, rather than direct causation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11(2), 167–203. PubMed ID: 18453461 doi:10.1177/1088868307301033
Bhangal, S., Cho, H., Geisler, M.W., & Morsella, E. (2016). The prospective nature of voluntary action: Insights from the Reflexive Imagery Task. Review of General Psychology, 20(1), 101–117. doi:10.1037/gpr0000071
Bhangal, S., Merrick, C., Cho, H., & Morsella, E. (2018). Involuntary entry into consciousness from the activation of sets: Object counting and color naming. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1017. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01017
Bhangal, S., Merrick, C., & Morsella, E. (2015). Ironic effects as reflexive responses: Evidence from word frequency effects on involuntary subvocalizations. Acta Psychologica, 159, 33–40. PubMed ID: 26005914 doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.04.006
Bindra, D. (1974). A motivational view of learning, performance, and behavior modification. Psychological Review, 81(3), 199–213. PubMed ID: 4424766 doi:10.1037/h0036330
Bindra, D. (1978). How adaptive behavior is produced: A perceptual-motivational alternative to response-reinforcement. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(1), 41–91. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00059380
Blanken, G., Wallesch, C, -W., & Papagno, C. (1990). Dissociations of language functions in aphasics with speech automatisms (recurring utterances). Cortex, 26(1), 41–63. PubMed ID: 2354645 doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(13)80074-3
Bryon, S., & Jedynak, C.P. (1972). Troubles du transfert interhemispherique: A propos de trois observations de tumeurs du corps calleux. Le signe de la main etrangère. Revue Neurologique, 126(4), 257–266.
Bui, N.-C.T., Ghafur, R.D., Yankulova, J., & Morsella, E. (2019). Stimulus-elicited involuntary insights and syntactic processing. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice. Advance online publication. doi:10.1037/cns0000208
Carlson, N.R. (1994). Physiology of behavior. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn and Bacon.
Carpenter, W.B. (1874). Principles of mental physiology. New York, NY: Appleton.
Chan, J.-L., & Ross, E.D. (1997). Alien hand syndrome: Influence of neglect on the clinical presentation of frontal and callosal variants. Cortex, 33(2), 287–299. PubMed ID: 9220259 doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70005-4
Cho, H., Zarolia, P., Gazzaley, A., & Morsella, E. (2016). Involuntary symbol manipulation (Pig Latin) from external control: Implications for thought suppression. Acta Psychologica, 166, 37–41. PubMed ID: 27043254 doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.03.004
Chomsky, N. (1988). Language and problems of knowledge: The Managua lectures. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Cushing, D., Gazzaley, A., & Morsella, E.. (2017). Externally controlled involuntary cognitions and their relations with other representations in consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 55, 1–10.
Cushing, D., Gazzaley, A., & Morsella, E. (2019). Involuntary mental rotation and visuospatial imagery from external control. Consciousness and Cognition, 75, 102809. PubMed ID: 31522028 doi:10.1016/j.concog.2019.102809
Cushing, D., Ghafur, R.D., & Morsella, E. (2017). The interdependence between conscious and unconscious processes. In Z. Radman (Ed.), Before consciousness: In search of the fundamentals of mind (pp. 50–82). Exeter, UK: Imprint Academic.
Dehaene, S. (2014). Consciousness and the brain: Deciphering how the brain codes our thoughts. New York, NY: Viking.
Dehaene S., & Naccache L. (2001). Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: Basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition, 79(1–2), 1–37. PubMed ID: 11164022 doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00123-2
Desender, K., van Opstal, F.V., & van den Bussche, E. (2014). Feeling the conflict: The crucial role of conflict experience in adaptation. Psychological Science, 25(3), 675–683. PubMed ID: 24395737 doi:10.1177/0956797613511468
de Waal, F.B.M. (2002). Evolutionary psychology: The wheat and the chaff. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(6), 187–191. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00197
Doesburg, S.M., Green, J.L., McDonald, J.J., & Ward, L.M. (2009). Rhythms of consciousness: Binocular rivalry reveals large-scale oscillatory network dynamics mediating visual perception. PLoS, 4(7), 1–14. PubMed ID: 19582165 doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0006142
Doherty, M.J., Wilensky A.J., Holmes, M.D., Lewis, D.H., Rae, J., & Cohn, G.H. (2002). Singing seizures. Neurology, 59(9), 1435–1438. PubMed ID: 12427900 doi:10.1212/01.WNL.0000031430.40085.93
Dou, W., Allen, A.K., Cho, H., Bhangal, S., Cook, A.J., Morsella, E., & Geisler, M.W. (2020). EEG correlates of involuntary cognitions in the Reflexive Imagery Task. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 482. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00482
Dou, W., Walker, E.B., & Morsella, E. (2020). The prospective nature of involuntary entry into consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies. 27(3–4), 69–87.
Duverne, S., & Koechlin, E. (2017). Hierarchical control of behaviour in human prefrontal cortex. In T. Enger (Ed.), The Wiley handbook of cognitive control (pp. 207–220). Oxford, UK: Wiley Blackwell.
Enatsu, R., Hantus, S., Gonzalez-Martinez, J., & So, N. (2011). Ictal singing due to left frontal lobe epilepsy: A case report and review of the literature. Epilepsy & Behavior, 22(2), 404–406. PubMed ID: 21889414 doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2011.07.019
Eriksen, B.A., & Eriksen, C.W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception and Psychophysics, 16(1), 143–149. doi:10.3758/BF03203267
Eriksen, C.W., & Schultz, D.W. (1979). Information processing in visual search: A continuous flow conception and experimental results. Perception and Psychophysics, 25(4), 249–263. PubMed ID: 461085 doi:10.3758/BF03198804
Firestone, C., & Scholl, B.J. (2014). “Top-down” effects where none should be found: The El Greco fallacy in perception research. Psychological Science, 25(1), 38–46. PubMed ID: 24297777 doi:10.1177/0956797613485092
Firestone, C., & Scholl, B.J. (2016). Cognition does not affect perception: Evaluating the evidence for ‘top-down’ effects [Target article]. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, 1–77.
Freeman, W.J. (2004). William James on consciousness, revisited. Chaos and Complexity Letters, 1(1), 17–42.
Freud, S. (1938). (A.A. Brill, ed. & Trans.). The basic writings of Sigmund Freud. New York, NY: Modern Library.
Frith, C.D. (2010). What is consciousness for? Pragmatics & Cognition, 18, 497–551.
Frith, C.D., Blakemore, S.J., & Wolpert, D.M. (2000). Abnormalities in the awareness and control of action. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 355(1404), 1771–1788. PubMed ID: 11205340 doi:10.1098/rstb.2000.0734
Gardner, K., Walker, E.B., Li, Y., Gazzaley, A., & Morsella, E. (2020). Involuntary attentional shifts as a function of set and processing fluency. Acta Psychologica, 203, 103009. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2020.103009
Gibson, J.J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
Gollwitzer, P.M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493–503.
Gould, S.J. (1977). Ever since Darwin: Reflections in natural history. New York, NY: Norton.
Grafman, J., & Krueger, F. (2009). The prefrontal cortex stores structured event complexes that are the representational basis for cognitively derived actions. In E. Morsella, J.A. Bargh, & P.M. Gollwitzer (Eds.), Oxford handbook of human action (pp. 197–213). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Graziano, M.S.A. (2013). Consciousness and the social brain. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Greenwald, A.G. (1970). Sensory feedback mechanisms in performance control: With special reference to the ideo-motor mechanism. Psychological Review, 77(2), 73–99. PubMed ID: 5454129 doi:10.1037/h0028689
Greenwald, A.G., & Pratkanis, A.R. (1984). The self. In R.S. Wyer & T.K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (pp. 129–178). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Grossberg, S. (1999). The link between brain learning, attention, and consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 8(1), 1–44. PubMed ID: 10072692 doi:10.1006/ccog.1998.0372
Harleß, E. (1861). Der apparat des willens [The apparatus of the will]. Zeitshrift für Philosophie und philosophische Kritik, 38, 499–507.
Helmholtz, H.V. (1856/1961). Treatise of physiological optics: Concerning the perceptions in general. In T. Shipley (Ed.), Classics in psychology (pp. 79–127). New York, NY: Philosophy Library.
Hesslow, G. (2002). Conscious thought as simulation of behavior and perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(6), 242–247. PubMed ID: 12039605 doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01913-7
Hommel, B. (2009). Action control according to TEC (theory of event coding). Psychological Research, 73(4), 512–526. PubMed ID: 19337749 doi:10.1007/s00426-009-0234-2
Hommel, B. (2013). Dancing in the dark: No role for consciousness in action control. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 380. PubMed ID: 23805123 doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00380
Hommel, B. (2015). The theory of event coding (TEC) as embodied-cognition framework. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1318. PubMed ID: 26388819 doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01318
Hommel, B., & Elsner, B. (2009). Acquisition, representation, and control of action. In E. Morsella, J.A. Bargh, & P.M. Gollwitzer (Eds.), Oxford handbook of human action (pp. 371–398). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding: A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(5), 849–878. PubMed ID: 12239891 doi:10.1017/S0140525X01000103
Jackson, F. (1986). What Mary didn’t know. Journal of Philosophy, 83(5), 291–295. doi:10.2307/2026143
James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York, NY: Dover.
Jantz, T.K., Tomory, J.J., Merrick, C., Cooper, S., Gazzaley, A., & Morsella, E. (2014). Subjective aspects of working memory performance: Memoranda-related imagery. Consciousness and Cognition, 25, 88–100. PubMed ID: 24583457 doi:10.1016/j.concog.2014.01.013
Jaynes, J. (1976). The origin of consciousness in the break-down of the bicameral mind. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
Jeannerod, M. (2006). Motor cognition: What action tells the self. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Johnson, M.K., & Raye, C.L. (1981). Reality monitoring. Psychological Review, 88(1), 67–85. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.88.1.67
Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York, NY: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.
Kaido, T., Otsuki, T., Nakama, H., Kaneko, Y., Kubota, Y., Sugai, K., & Saito, O. (2006). Complex behavioral automatism arising from insular cortex. Epilepsy and Behavior, 8(1), 315–319. PubMed ID: 16356775 doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2005.10.006
Kececi, H., Degirmenci, Y., & Gumus, H. (2013). Two foreign language automatisms in complex partial seizures. Epilepsy and Behavior Case Reports, 1, 7–9. PubMed ID: 25688045 doi:10.1016/j.ebcr.2012.10.005
Kinsbourne, M. (1996). What qualifies a representation for a role in consciousness? In J.D. Cohen & J.W. Schooler (Eds.), Scientific approaches to consciousness (pp. 335–355). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.
Kinsbourne, M. (2000). How is consciousness expressed in the cerebral activation manifold? Brain and Mind, 1(2), 265–274. doi:10.1023/A:1010041218568
Koch, C. (2004). The quest for consciousness: A neurobiological approach. Englewood, CO: Roberts & Company.
Köhler, W. (1947). Gestalt psychology: An introduction to new concepts in modern psychology. New York, NY: Liveright Publishing.
Kokkinos, V., Zountsas, B., Kontogiannis, K., & Garganis, K. (2012). Epileptogenic networks in two patients with hypothalamic hamartoma. Brain Topography, 25(3), 327–331. PubMed ID: 22207194 doi:10.1007/s10548-011-0214-x
Kouider, S., & Dupoux, E. (2004). Partial awareness creates the “illusion” of subliminal semantic priming. Psychological Science, 15(2), 75–81. PubMed ID: 14738512 doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01502001.x
Kutlu, G., Bilir, E., Erdem, A., Gomceli, Y.B., Kurt, G.S., & Serdaroglu, A. (2005). Hush sign: A new clinical sign in temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior, 6(3), 452–455. PubMed ID: 15820360 doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2005.01.006
Lashley, K.S. (1923). The behavioristic interpretation of consciousness. Psychological Bulletin, 30(4), 237–272 (Part 1), 329–353 (Part 2).
Lashley, K.S. (1942). The problem of cerebral organization in vision. In H. Kluver (Ed.), Visual mechanisms: Biological symposia, 7 (pp. 301–322). Lancaster, PA: Cattell Press.
Lashley, K.S. (1956). Cerebral organization and behavior. Proceedings of the Association for Research in Nervouse and Mental Diseases, 36, 1–18.
Lehar, S. (2003). Gestalt isomorphism and the primacy of the subjective conscious experience: A gestalt bubble model. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26(4), 375–408. PubMed ID: 15067950 doi:10.1017/S0140525X03000098
Levelt, W.J.M. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Lewin, K. (1935). A dynamic theory of personality. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Lhermitte, F. (1983). ‘Utilization behavior’ and its relation to lesions of the frontal lobes. Brain, 106(Pt. 2), 237–255. PubMed ID: 6850269 doi:10.1093/brain/106.2.237
Lieberman, M.D. (2007). The X- and C-systems: The neural basis of automatic and controlled social cognition. In E. Harmon-Jones & P. Winkielman (Eds.), Fundamentals of social neuroscience (pp. 290–315). New York, NY: Guilford.
Livnat, A., & Pippenger, N. (2006). An optimal brain can be composed of conflicting agents. Proceeding of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 103(9), 3198–3202. doi:10.1073/pnas.0510932103
Logothetis, N.K., & Schall, J.D. (1989). Neuronal correlates of subjective visual perception. Science, 245(4919), 761–763. PubMed ID: 2772635 doi:10.1126/science.2772635
Lorenz, K. (1963). On aggression. New York, NY: Harcourt, Brace, & World.
Lotze, R.H. (1852). Medizinische Psychologie oder Physiologie der Seele. Leipzig, Germany: Weidmann’sche Buchhandlung.
Macphail, E.M. (1998). The evolution of consciousness. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Marchetti, C., & Della Sala, S. (1998). Disentangling the alien and anarchic hand. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 3(3), 191–207. doi:10.1080/135468098396143
Marcus, G. (2008). Kluge: The haphazard construction of the mind. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.
Masicampo, E.J., & Baumeister, R.F. (2013). Conscious thought does not guide moment-to-moment actions—it serves social and cultural functions. Frontiers in Psycholology, 4, 478. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00478
McClelland, J.L. (1979). On the time-relations of mental processes: An examination of systems of processes in cascade. Psychological Review, 86(4), 287–330. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.86.4.287
McGurk, H., & MacDonald, J. (1976). Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature, 264(5588), 746–748. PubMed ID: 1012311 doi:10.1038/264746a0
Mechsner, F., Kerzel, D., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). Perceptual basis of bimanual coordination. Nature, 414(6859), 69–73. PubMed ID: 11689944 doi:10.1038/35102060
Merker, B. (2007). Consciousness without a cerebral cortex: A challenge for neuroscience and medicine. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(1), 63–81. PubMed ID: 17475053 doi:10.1017/S0140525X07000891
Merker, B. (2012). From probabilities to percepts: A subcortical “global best estimatebuffer” as locus of phenomenal experience. In E. Shimon, T. Fekete, & N. Zach (Eds.), Being in time: Dynamical models of phenomenal experience. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: John Benjamins.
Merker, B. (2013). The efference cascade, consciousness, and its self: Naturalizing the first person pivot of action control. Frontiers in Psycholology, 4, 501. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00501
Merrick, C., Farnia, M., Jantz, T.K., Gazzaley, A., & Morsella, E. (2015). External control of the stream of consciousness: Stimulus-based effects on involuntary thought sequences. Consciousness and Cognition, 33, 217–225. PubMed ID: 25636006 doi:10.1016/j.concog.2014.12.014
Miller, B.L., & Cummings, J.L. (2007). The human frontal lobes: Functions and disorders (2nd ed.) New York, NY: Guilford Press.
Miller, E.K. (2000). The prefrontal cortex and cognitive control. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 1(1), 59–65. PubMed ID: 11252769 doi:10.1038/35036228
Miller, E.K., & Wallis, J.D. (2009). Executive function and higher-order cognition: Definitions and neural substrates. In L.R. Squire (Ed.), Encyclopedia of neuroscience (Vol. 4, pp. 99–104). Oxford, UK: Academic Press.
Miller, G.A. (1962). Psychology: The science of mental life. New York, NY: Adams, Bannister, & Cox.
Miller, N.E. (1959). Liberalization of basic S-R concepts: Extensions to conflict behavior, motivation, and social learning. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science (Vol. 2, pp. 196–292). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.
Milner, A.D., & Goodale, M. (1995). The visual brain in action. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Mitchell, A.S., Baxter, M.G., & Gaffan, D. (2007). Dissociable performance on scene learning and strategy implementation after lesions to magnocellular mediodorsal thalamic nucleus. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27(44), 11888–11895. PubMed ID: 17978029 doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1835-07.2007
Morsella, E. (2005). The function of phenomenal states: Supramodular interaction theory. Psychological Review, 112(4), 1000–1021. PubMed ID: 16262477 doi:10.1037/0033-295X.112.4.1000
Morsella, E., & Bargh, J.A. (2011). Unconscious action tendencies: Sources of ‘un-integrated’ action. In J.T. Cacioppo & J. Decety (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of social neuroscience (pp. 335–347). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Morsella, E., Godwin, C.A., Jantz, T.K., Krieger, S.C., & Gazzaley, A. (2016). Homing in on consciousness in the nervous system: An action-based synthesis [Target article]. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, 1–17.
Morsella, E., Gray, J.R., Krieger, S.C., & Bargh, J.A. (2009a). The essence of conscious conflict: Subjective effects of sustaining incompatible intentions. Emotion, 9(5), 717–728. doi:10.1037/a0017121
Morsella, E., & Krauss, R.M. (2004). The role of gestures in spatial working memory and speech. American Journal of Psychology, 117(3), 411–424. PubMed ID: 15457809 doi:10.2307/4149008
Morsella, E., Lanska, M., Berger, C.C., & Gazzaley, A. (2009c). Indirect cognitive control through top-down activation of perceptual symbols. European Journal of Social Psychology, 39(7), 1173–1177. doi:10.1002/ejsp.679
Morsella, E., & Miozzo, M. (2002). Evidence for a cascade model of lexical access in speech production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(3), 555–563. PubMed ID: 12018507
Morsella, E., & Poehlman, T.A. (2013). The inevitable contrast: Conscious versus unconscious processes in action control. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 590. PubMed ID: 24058351 doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00590
Morsella, E., Wilson, L.E., Berger, C.C., Honhongva, M., Gazzaley, A., & Bargh, J.A. (2009b). Subjective aspects of cognitive control at different stages of processing. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71(8), 1807–1824. doi:10.3758/APP.71.8.1807
Mudrik, L., Faivre, N., & Koch, C. (2014). Information integration without awareness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(9), 488–496. PubMed ID: 24933626 doi:10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.009
Munakata, Y., Herd, S.A., Chatham, C.H., Depue, B.E., Banich, M.T., & O’Reilly, R.C. (2011). A unified framework for inhibitory control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(10), 453–459. PubMed ID: 21889391 doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.07.011
Muzur, A., Pace-Schott, E.F., & Hobson, J.A. (2002). The prefrontal cortex in sleep. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(11), 475–481. PubMed ID: 12457899 doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01992-7
O’Reilly, R.C. (2010). The what and how of prefrontal cortical organization. Trends in Neurosciences, 33(8), 355–361. PubMed ID: 20573407 doi:10.1016/j.tins.2010.05.002
Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. New York, NY: Norton.
Prinz, J. (2007). The intermediate level theory of consciousness. In M. Velmans & S. Schneider (Eds.), The Blackwell companion to consciousness (pp. 248–260). Malden, MA: Blackwell.
Prinz, W. (2003). How do we know about our own actions? In S. Maasen, W. Prinz, & G. Roth (Eds.), Voluntary action: Brains, minds, and sociality (pp. 21–33). London, UK: Oxford University Press.
Prinz, W. (2012). Open minds: The social making of agency and intentionality. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Prinz, W., Aschersleben, G., & Koch, I. (2009). Cognition and action. In E. Morsella, J.A. Bargh, & P.M. Gollwitzer (Eds.), Oxford handbook of human action (pp. 35–71). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Proctor, R.W., & Vu, K.-P.L. (2010). Action selection. In I.B. Weiner & E. Craighead (Eds.), The Corsini encyclopedia of psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 20–22). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley.
Pylyshyn, Z.W. (1984). Computation and cognition: Toward a foundation for cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Questienne, L., Atas, A., Burle, B., & Gevers, W. (2018). Objectifying the subjective: Building blocks of metacognitive experiences in conflict tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(1), 125–131. doi:10.1037/xge0000370
Rolls, E.T., Judge, S.J., & Sanghera, M. (1977). Activity of neurons in the inferotemporal cortex of the alert monkey. Brain Research, 130(2), 229–238. PubMed ID: 406968 doi:10.1016/0006-8993(77)90272-4
Rosenbaum, D.A. (2002). Motor control. In H. Pashler (Series Ed.) & S. Yantis (Vol. Ed.), Stevens’ handbook of experimental psychology: Sensation and perception (3rd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 315–339). New York, NY: Wiley.
Roser, M., & Gazzaniga, M.S. (2004). Automatic brains–interpretive minds. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(2), 56–59. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00274.x
Rumelhart, D.E., McClelland, J.L., & the PDP Research Group. (1986). Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition (Vols. 1 & 2). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Rumelhart, D.E., & Norman, D.A. (1988). Representation in memory. In R. Atkinson, D. Luce, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Revised Stevens handbook of experimental psychology (pp. 511–587). New York, NY: Wiley.
Schacter, D.L. (1996). Searching for memory: The brain, the mind, and the past. New York, NY: Basic Books.
Scott, M. (2013). Corollary discharge provides the sensory content of inner speech. Psychological Science, 24(9), 1824–1830. PubMed ID: 23846719 doi:10.1177/0956797613478614
Searle, J.R. (2000). Consciousness. Annual Review of Neurosciences, 23, 557–578. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.557
Simons, D.J., & Levin, D.T. (1997). Change blindness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1(7), 261–267. PubMed ID: 21223921 doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(97)01080-2
Simpson, G.G. (1949). The meaning of evolution. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.
Skinner, B.F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York, NY: Macmillan.
Spear, N.E., & Campbell, B.A. (1979). Ontogeny of learning and memory. New York, NY: Lawrence Erlbaum.
Squire, L.R. (1987). Memory and brain. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Stevens, S.S. (1975). Psychophysics: Introduction to its perceptual, neural, and social prospects. New York, NY: Wiley.
Stroop, J.R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643–662. doi:10.1037/h0054651
Stuss, D.T., & Anderson, V. (2004). The frontal lobes and theory of mind: Developmental concepts from adult focal lesion research. Brain & Cognition, 55(1), 69–83. PubMed ID: 15134844 doi:10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00271-9
Suzuki, T., Itoh, S., Arai, N., Kouno, M., Noguchi, M., Takatsu, M., & Takeda, K. (2012). Ambient echolalia in a patient with germinoma around the bilateral ventriculus lateralis: A case report. Neurocase, 18(4), 330–335. PubMed ID: 22117108 doi:10.1080/13554794.2011.608364
Taylor, J.A., & Ivry, R.B. (2013). Implicit and explicit processes in motor learning. In W. Prinz, M. Beisert, & A. Herwig (Eds.), Action science (pp. 63–87). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Thagard, P., & Stewart, T.C. (2014). Two theories of consciousness: Semantic pointer competition vs. information integration. Consciousness and Cognition, 30, 73–90. PubMed ID: 25160821 doi:10.1016/j.concog.2014.07.001
Titchener, E.B. (1896). An outline of psychology. New York, NY: Macmillan.
Tortora, G.J. (1994). Introduction to the human body: The essentials of anatomy and physiology (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Harper Collins.
Tsotsos, J.K. (1995). Behaviorist intelligence and the scaling problem. Artificial Intelligence, 75(2), 135–160. doi:10.1016/0004-3702(94)00019-W
Tsotsos, J.K. (2011). A computational perspective on visual attention. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.
Vroomen, J., & de Gelder, B. (2003). Visual motion influences the contingent auditory motion aftereffect. Psychological Science, 14(4), 357–361. PubMed ID: 12807410 doi:10.1111/1467-9280.24431
Wegner, D.M. (1989). White bears and other unwanted thoughts. New York, NY: Viking/Penguin.
Weiskrantz, L. (1992). Unconscious vision: The strange phenomenon of blindsight. The Sciences, 35, 23–28.
Weiskrantz, L. (1997). Consciousness lost and found: A neuropsychological exploration. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
Westwood, D.A. (2009). The visual control of object manipulation. In E. Morsella, J.A. Bargh, & P.M. Gollwitzer (Eds.), Oxford handbook of human action (pp. 88–103). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.
White, N.A., Velasquez, A.G., & Morsella, E. (2018). Mary had a little . . . : Involuntary music imagery and memory retrieval. Poster presented at the Annual Convention of the Association for Psychological Science, San Francisco, CA.
Yamadori, A. (1997). Body awareness and its disorders. In M. Ito, Y. Miyashita, Y., & E.T. Rolls (Eds.), Cognition, computation, and consciousness (pp. 169–176). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.
Yankulova, J.K., & Morsella, E. (2020). Conscious contents: Their unanalyzable, arbitrary, and unarbitrary properties. Cognitive Neuropsychology. Advance online publication. doi:10.1080/02643294.2020.1728242
Yates, J. (1985). The content of awareness is a model of the world. Psychological Review, 92(2), 249–284. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.92.2.249
All Time | Past Year | Past 30 Days | |
---|---|---|---|
Abstract Views | 1254 | 1254 | 248 |
Full Text Views | 11 | 11 | 0 |
PDF Downloads | 8 | 8 | 0 |
© 2020 Human Kinetics
Powered by: