Motor Cognition: The Role of Sentience in Perception and Action

in Kinesiology Review

Click name to view affiliation

Ezequiel Morsella
Search for other papers by Ezequiel Morsella in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Anthony G. Velasquez
Search for other papers by Anthony G. Velasquez in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Jessica K. Yankulova
Search for other papers by Jessica K. Yankulova in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Yanming Li
Search for other papers by Yanming Li in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
,
Christina Y. Wong
Search for other papers by Christina Y. Wong in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
, and
Dennis Lambert
Search for other papers by Dennis Lambert in
Current site
Google Scholar
PubMed
Close
Restricted access

The function of the conscious field remains mysterious from a scientific point of view. This article reviews theoretical approaches (passive frame theory and ideomotor approaches) that elucidate how the conscious field is intimately related to a special kind of action selection. This form of action selection is peculiar to the skeletal-muscle output system. The notion of encapsulation and how it explains many properties of the conscious field are discussed, including why the conscious field, though in the service of adaptive action, contains contents that are not action-relevant; why the field has a first-person perspective; and why the field is so thorough, in terms of its contents, the contrasts among contents, and the representation of spatial layout. The authors discuss subordinate encapsulation and the hypothesis that the conscious field is what allows for encapsulated conscious contents to influence action selection collectively, yielding what in everyday life is called voluntary behavior.

The authors are with the Dept. of Psychology, San Francisco State University, San Francisco, CA, USA. Morsella is also with the Dept. of Neurology, University of California, San Francisco, San Francisco, CA, USA.

Morsella (morsella@sfsu.edu) is corresponding author.
  • Collapse
  • Expand
  • Ach, N. (1905/1951). Determining tendencies: Awareness. In D. Rapaport (Ed.), Organization and pathology of thought (pp. 1538), New York, NY: Columbia University Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Agre, P.E., & Chapman, D. (1990). What are plans for? Robotics and Autonomous Systems, 6(1–2), 1734. doi:10.1016/S0921-8890(05)80026-0

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Allen, A.K., Krisst, L., Montemayor, C., & Morsella, E. (2016). Entry of involuntary conscious contents from ambiguous images. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice, 3(4), 326337. doi:10.1037/cns0000095

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Allen, A.K., Wilkins, K., Gazzaley, A., & Morsella, E. (2013). Conscious thoughts from reflex-like processes: A new experimental paradigm for consciousness research. Consciousness and Cognition, 22(4), 13181331. PubMed ID: 24056177 doi:10.1016/j.concog.2013.08.007

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Anderson, M.C., Ochsner, K.N., Kuhl, B., Cooper, J., Robertson, E., Gabrieli, S.W., . . . Gabrieli, J.D.E. (2004). Neural systems underlying the suppression of unwanted memories. Science, 303(5655), 232235. PubMed ID: 14716015 doi:10.1126/science.1089504

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Arkin, R.C. (1998). Behavior-based robotics. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  • Baars, B.J. (1988). A cognitive theory of consciousness. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press.

  • Badets, A., & Osiurak, F. (2015). A goal-based mechanism for delayed motor intention: Considerations from motor skills, tool use and action memory. Psychological Research, 79(3), 345360. PubMed ID: 24913016 doi:10.1007/s00426-014-0581-5

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Badre, D. (2008). Cognitive control, hierarchy, and the rostro-caudal organization of the frontal lobes. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 12(5), 193200. PubMed ID: 18403252 doi:10.1016/j.tics.2008.02.004

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Badre, D., & D’Esposito, M. (2007). Functional magnetic resonance imaging evidence for a hierarchical organization of the prefrontal cortex. Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 19(12), 20822099. PubMed ID: 17892391 doi:10.1162/jocn.2007.19.12.2082

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bahlmann, J., Blumenfeld, R.S., & D’Esposito, M. (2015). The rostro-caudal axis of frontal cortex is sensitive to the domain of stimulus information. Cerebral Cortex, 25(7), 18151826. PubMed ID: 24451658 doi:10.1093/cercor/bht419

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Banks, W.P. (1995). Evidence for consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 4(2), 270272. PubMed ID: 8521269 doi:10.1006/ccog.1995.1037

  • Bargh, J.A., & Morsella, E. (2008). The unconscious mind. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3(1), 7379. PubMed ID: 18584056 doi:10.1111/j.1745-6916.2008.00064.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Baumeister, R.F., Vohs, K.D., DeWall, N., & Zhang, L. (2007). How emotion shapes behavior: Feedback, anticipation, and reflection, rather than direct causation. Personality and Social Psychology Review, 11(2), 167203. PubMed ID: 18453461 doi:10.1177/1088868307301033

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bhangal, S., Cho, H., Geisler, M.W., & Morsella, E. (2016). The prospective nature of voluntary action: Insights from the Reflexive Imagery Task. Review of General Psychology, 20(1), 101117. doi:10.1037/gpr0000071

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bhangal, S., Merrick, C., Cho, H., & Morsella, E. (2018). Involuntary entry into consciousness from the activation of sets: Object counting and color naming. Frontiers in Psychology, 9, 1017. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01017

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bhangal, S., Merrick, C., & Morsella, E. (2015). Ironic effects as reflexive responses: Evidence from word frequency effects on involuntary subvocalizations. Acta Psychologica, 159, 3340. PubMed ID: 26005914 doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2015.04.006

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bindra, D. (1974). A motivational view of learning, performance, and behavior modification. Psychological Review, 81(3), 199213. PubMed ID: 4424766 doi:10.1037/h0036330

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bindra, D. (1978). How adaptive behavior is produced: A perceptual-motivational alternative to response-reinforcement. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 1(1), 4191. doi:10.1017/S0140525X00059380

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Blanken, G., Wallesch, C, -W., & Papagno, C. (1990). Dissociations of language functions in aphasics with speech automatisms (recurring utterances). Cortex, 26(1), 4163. PubMed ID: 2354645 doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(13)80074-3

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bryon, S., & Jedynak, C.P. (1972). Troubles du transfert interhemispherique: A propos de trois observations de tumeurs du corps calleux. Le signe de la main etrangère. Revue Neurologique, 126(4), 257266.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Bui, N.-C.T., Ghafur, R.D., Yankulova, J., & Morsella, E. (2019). Stimulus-elicited involuntary insights and syntactic processing. Psychology of Consciousness: Theory, Research, and Practice. Advance online publication. doi:10.1037/cns0000208

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Carlson, N.R. (1994). Physiology of behavior. Needham Heights, MA: Allyn and Bacon.

  • Carpenter, W.B. (1874). Principles of mental physiology. New York, NY: Appleton.

  • Chan, J.-L., & Ross, E.D. (1997). Alien hand syndrome: Influence of neglect on the clinical presentation of frontal and callosal variants. Cortex, 33(2), 287299. PubMed ID: 9220259 doi:10.1016/S0010-9452(08)70005-4

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cho, H., Zarolia, P., Gazzaley, A., & Morsella, E. (2016). Involuntary symbol manipulation (Pig Latin) from external control: Implications for thought suppression. Acta Psychologica, 166, 3741. PubMed ID: 27043254 doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.03.004

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Chomsky, N. (1988). Language and problems of knowledge: The Managua lectures. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  • Cushing, D., Gazzaley, A., & Morsella, E.. (2017). Externally controlled involuntary cognitions and their relations with other representations in consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 55, 110.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cushing, D., Gazzaley, A., & Morsella, E. (2019). Involuntary mental rotation and visuospatial imagery from external control. Consciousness and Cognition, 75, 102809. PubMed ID: 31522028 doi:10.1016/j.concog.2019.102809

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Cushing, D., Ghafur, R.D., & Morsella, E. (2017). The interdependence between conscious and unconscious processes. In Z. Radman (Ed.), Before consciousness: In search of the fundamentals of mind (pp. 5082). Exeter, UK: Imprint Academic.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dehaene, S. (2014). Consciousness and the brain: Deciphering how the brain codes our thoughts. New York, NY: Viking.

  • Dehaene S., & Naccache L. (2001). Towards a cognitive neuroscience of consciousness: Basic evidence and a workspace framework. Cognition, 79(1–2), 137. PubMed ID: 11164022 doi:10.1016/S0010-0277(00)00123-2

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Desender, K., van Opstal, F.V., & van den Bussche, E. (2014). Feeling the conflict: The crucial role of conflict experience in adaptation. Psychological Science, 25(3), 675683. PubMed ID: 24395737 doi:10.1177/0956797613511468

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • de Waal, F.B.M. (2002). Evolutionary psychology: The wheat and the chaff. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 11(6), 187191. doi:10.1111/1467-8721.00197

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Doesburg, S.M., Green, J.L., McDonald, J.J., & Ward, L.M. (2009). Rhythms of consciousness: Binocular rivalry reveals large-scale oscillatory network dynamics mediating visual perception. PLoS, 4(7), 114. PubMed ID: 19582165 doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0006142

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Doherty, M.J., Wilensky A.J., Holmes, M.D., Lewis, D.H., Rae, J., & Cohn, G.H. (2002). Singing seizures. Neurology, 59(9), 14351438. PubMed ID: 12427900 doi:10.1212/01.WNL.0000031430.40085.93

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dou, W., Allen, A.K., Cho, H., Bhangal, S., Cook, A.J., Morsella, E., & Geisler, M.W. (2020). EEG correlates of involuntary cognitions in the Reflexive Imagery Task. Frontiers in Psychology, 11, 482. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2020.00482

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Dou, W., Walker, E.B., & Morsella, E. (2020). The prospective nature of involuntary entry into consciousness. Journal of Consciousness Studies. 27(3–4), 6987.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Duverne, S., & Koechlin, E. (2017). Hierarchical control of behaviour in human prefrontal cortex. In T. Enger (Ed.), The Wiley handbook of cognitive control (pp. 207220). Oxford, UK: Wiley Blackwell.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Enatsu, R., Hantus, S., Gonzalez-Martinez, J., & So, N. (2011). Ictal singing due to left frontal lobe epilepsy: A case report and review of the literature. Epilepsy & Behavior, 22(2), 404406. PubMed ID: 21889414 doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2011.07.019

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Eriksen, B.A., & Eriksen, C.W. (1974). Effects of noise letters upon the identification of a target letter in a nonsearch task. Perception and Psychophysics, 16(1), 143149. doi:10.3758/BF03203267

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Eriksen, C.W., & Schultz, D.W. (1979). Information processing in visual search: A continuous flow conception and experimental results. Perception and Psychophysics, 25(4), 249263. PubMed ID: 461085 doi:10.3758/BF03198804

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Firestone, C., & Scholl, B.J. (2014). “Top-down” effects where none should be found: The El Greco fallacy in perception research. Psychological Science, 25(1), 3846. PubMed ID: 24297777 doi:10.1177/0956797613485092

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Firestone, C., & Scholl, B.J. (2016). Cognition does not affect perception: Evaluating the evidence for ‘top-down’ effects [Target article]. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, 177.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Freeman, W.J. (2004). William James on consciousness, revisited. Chaos and Complexity Letters, 1(1), 1742.

  • Freud, S. (1938). (A.A. Brill, ed. & Trans.). The basic writings of Sigmund Freud. New York, NYModern Library.

  • Frith, C.D. (2010). What is consciousness for? Pragmatics & Cognition, 18, 497551.

  • Frith, C.D., Blakemore, S.J., & Wolpert, D.M. (2000). Abnormalities in the awareness and control of action. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society of London, 355(1404), 17711788. PubMed ID: 11205340 doi:10.1098/rstb.2000.0734

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gardner, K., Walker, E.B., Li, Y., Gazzaley, A., & Morsella, E. (2020). Involuntary attentional shifts as a function of set and processing fluency. Acta Psychologica, 203, 103009. doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2020.103009

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Gibson, J.J. (1979). The ecological approach to visual perception. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.

  • Gollwitzer, P.M. (1999). Implementation intentions: Strong effects of simple plans. American Psychologist, 54, 493503.

  • Gould, S.J. (1977). Ever since Darwin: Reflections in natural history. New York, NY: Norton.

  • Grafman, J., & Krueger, F. (2009). The prefrontal cortex stores structured event complexes that are the representational basis for cognitively derived actions. In E. Morsella, J.A. Bargh, & P.M. Gollwitzer (Eds.), Oxford handbook of human action (pp. 197213). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Graziano, M.S.A. (2013). Consciousness and the social brain. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

  • Greenwald, A.G. (1970). Sensory feedback mechanisms in performance control: With special reference to the ideo-motor mechanism. Psychological Review, 77(2), 7399. PubMed ID: 5454129 doi:10.1037/h0028689

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Greenwald, A.G., & Pratkanis, A.R. (1984). The self. In R.S. Wyer & T.K. Srull (Eds.), Handbook of social cognition (pp. 129178). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Grossberg, S. (1999). The link between brain learning, attention, and consciousness. Consciousness and Cognition, 8(1), 144. PubMed ID: 10072692 doi:10.1006/ccog.1998.0372

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Harleß, E. (1861). Der apparat des willens [The apparatus of the will]. Zeitshrift für Philosophie und philosophische Kritik, 38, 499507.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Helmholtz, H.V. (1856/1961). Treatise of physiological optics: Concerning the perceptions in general. In T. Shipley (Ed.), Classics in psychology (pp. 79127). New York, NY: Philosophy Library.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hesslow, G. (2002). Conscious thought as simulation of behavior and perception. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(6), 242247. PubMed ID: 12039605 doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01913-7

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hommel, B. (2009). Action control according to TEC (theory of event coding). Psychological Research, 73(4), 512526. PubMed ID: 19337749 doi:10.1007/s00426-009-0234-2

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hommel, B. (2013). Dancing in the dark: No role for consciousness in action control. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 380. PubMed ID: 23805123 doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00380

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hommel, B. (2015). The theory of event coding (TEC) as embodied-cognition framework. Frontiers in Psychology, 6, 1318. PubMed ID: 26388819 doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01318

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hommel, B., & Elsner, B. (2009). Acquisition, representation, and control of action. In E. Morsella, J.A. Bargh, & P.M. Gollwitzer (Eds.), Oxford handbook of human action (pp. 371398). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Hommel, B., Müsseler, J., Aschersleben, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). The theory of event coding: A framework for perception and action planning. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 24(5), 849878. PubMed ID: 12239891 doi:10.1017/S0140525X01000103

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jackson, F. (1986). What Mary didn’t know. Journal of Philosophy, 83(5), 291295. doi:10.2307/2026143

  • James, W. (1890). The principles of psychology. New York, NY: Dover.

  • Jantz, T.K., Tomory, J.J., Merrick, C., Cooper, S., Gazzaley, A., & Morsella, E. (2014). Subjective aspects of working memory performance: Memoranda-related imagery. Consciousness and Cognition, 25, 88100. PubMed ID: 24583457 doi:10.1016/j.concog.2014.01.013

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Jaynes, J. (1976). The origin of consciousness in the break-down of the bicameral mind. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.

  • Jeannerod, M. (2006). Motor cognition: What action tells the self. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

  • Johnson, M.K., & Raye, C.L. (1981). Reality monitoring. Psychological Review, 88(1), 6785. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.88.1.67

  • Kahneman, D. (2011). Thinking, fast and slow. New York, NY: Farrar, Straus and Giroux.

  • Kaido, T., Otsuki, T., Nakama, H., Kaneko, Y., Kubota, Y., Sugai, K., & Saito, O. (2006). Complex behavioral automatism arising from insular cortex. Epilepsy and Behavior, 8(1), 315319. PubMed ID: 16356775 doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2005.10.006

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kececi, H., Degirmenci, Y., & Gumus, H. (2013). Two foreign language automatisms in complex partial seizures. Epilepsy and Behavior Case Reports, 1, 79. PubMed ID: 25688045 doi:10.1016/j.ebcr.2012.10.005

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kinsbourne, M. (1996). What qualifies a representation for a role in consciousness? In J.D. Cohen & J.W. Schooler (Eds.), Scientific approaches to consciousness (pp. 335355). Hillsdale, NJ: Erlbaum.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kinsbourne, M. (2000). How is consciousness expressed in the cerebral activation manifold? Brain and Mind, 1(2), 265274. doi:10.1023/A:1010041218568

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Koch, C. (2004). The quest for consciousness: A neurobiological approach. Englewood, CO: Roberts & Company.

  • Köhler, W. (1947). Gestalt psychology: An introduction to new concepts in modern psychology. New York, NY: Liveright Publishing.

  • Kokkinos, V., Zountsas, B., Kontogiannis, K., & Garganis, K. (2012). Epileptogenic networks in two patients with hypothalamic hamartoma. Brain Topography, 25(3), 327331. PubMed ID: 22207194 doi:10.1007/s10548-011-0214-x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kouider, S., & Dupoux, E. (2004). Partial awareness creates the “illusion” of subliminal semantic priming. Psychological Science, 15(2), 7581. PubMed ID: 14738512 doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.01502001.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Kutlu, G., Bilir, E., Erdem, A., Gomceli, Y.B., Kurt, G.S., & Serdaroglu, A. (2005). Hush sign: A new clinical sign in temporal lobe epilepsy. Epilepsy and Behavior, 6(3), 452455. PubMed ID: 15820360 doi:10.1016/j.yebeh.2005.01.006

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lashley, K.S. (1923). The behavioristic interpretation of consciousness. Psychological Bulletin, 30(4), 237272 (Part 1), 329–353 (Part 2).

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lashley, K.S. (1942). The problem of cerebral organization in vision. In H. Kluver (Ed.), Visual mechanisms: Biological symposia, 7 (pp. 301322). Lancaster, PA: Cattell Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lashley, K.S. (1956). Cerebral organization and behavior. Proceedings of the Association for Research in Nervouse and Mental Diseases, 36, 118.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lehar, S. (2003). Gestalt isomorphism and the primacy of the subjective conscious experience: A gestalt bubble model. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 26(4), 375408. PubMed ID: 15067950 doi:10.1017/S0140525X03000098

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Levelt, W.J.M. (1989). Speaking: From intention to articulation. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  • Lewin, K. (1935). A dynamic theory of personality. New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

  • Lhermitte, F. (1983). ‘Utilization behavior’ and its relation to lesions of the frontal lobes. Brain, 106(Pt. 2), 237255. PubMed ID: 6850269 doi:10.1093/brain/106.2.237

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lieberman, M.D. (2007). The X- and C-systems: The neural basis of automatic and controlled social cognition. In E. Harmon-Jones & P. Winkielman (Eds.), Fundamentals of social neuroscience (pp. 290315). New York, NY: Guilford.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Livnat, A., & Pippenger, N. (2006). An optimal brain can be composed of conflicting agents. Proceeding of the National Academy of Sciences, USA, 103(9), 31983202. doi:10.1073/pnas.0510932103

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Logothetis, N.K., & Schall, J.D. (1989). Neuronal correlates of subjective visual perception. Science, 245(4919), 761763. PubMed ID: 2772635 doi:10.1126/science.2772635

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Lorenz, K. (1963). On aggression. New York, NY: Harcourt, Brace, & World.

  • Lotze, R.H. (1852). Medizinische Psychologie oder Physiologie der Seele. Leipzig, Germany: Weidmann’sche Buchhandlung.

  • Macphail, E.M. (1998). The evolution of consciousness. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

  • Marchetti, C., & Della Sala, S. (1998). Disentangling the alien and anarchic hand. Cognitive Neuropsychiatry, 3(3), 191207. doi:10.1080/135468098396143

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Marcus, G. (2008). Kluge: The haphazard construction of the mind. Boston, MA: Houghton Mifflin.

  • Masicampo, E.J., & Baumeister, R.F. (2013). Conscious thought does not guide moment-to-moment actions—it serves social and cultural functions. Frontiers in Psycholology, 4, 478. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00478

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • McClelland, J.L. (1979). On the time-relations of mental processes: An examination of systems of processes in cascade. Psychological Review, 86(4), 287330. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.86.4.287

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • McGurk, H., & MacDonald, J. (1976). Hearing lips and seeing voices. Nature, 264(5588), 746748. PubMed ID: 1012311 doi:10.1038/264746a0

  • Mechsner, F., Kerzel, D., Knoblich, G., & Prinz, W. (2001). Perceptual basis of bimanual coordination. Nature, 414(6859), 6973. PubMed ID: 11689944 doi:10.1038/35102060

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Merker, B. (2007). Consciousness without a cerebral cortex: A challenge for neuroscience and medicine. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 30(1), 6381. PubMed ID: 17475053 doi:10.1017/S0140525X07000891

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Merker, B. (2012). From probabilities to percepts: A subcortical “global best estimatebuffer” as locus of phenomenal experience. In E. Shimon, T. Fekete, & N. Zach (Eds.), Being in time: Dynamical models of phenomenal experience. Amsterdam, the Netherlands: John Benjamins.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Merker, B. (2013). The efference cascade, consciousness, and its self: Naturalizing the first person pivot of action control. Frontiers in Psycholology, 4, 501. doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00501

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Merrick, C., Farnia, M., Jantz, T.K., Gazzaley, A., & Morsella, E. (2015). External control of the stream of consciousness: Stimulus-based effects on involuntary thought sequences. Consciousness and Cognition, 33, 217225. PubMed ID: 25636006 doi:10.1016/j.concog.2014.12.014

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Miller, B.L., & Cummings, J.L. (2007). The human frontal lobes: Functions and disorders (2nd ed.) New York, NY: Guilford Press.

  • Miller, E.K. (2000). The prefrontal cortex and cognitive control. Nature Reviews Neuroscience, 1(1), 5965. PubMed ID: 11252769 doi:10.1038/35036228

  • Miller, E.K., & Wallis, J.D. (2009). Executive function and higher-order cognition: Definitions and neural substrates. In L.R. Squire (Ed.), Encyclopedia of neuroscience (Vol. 4, pp. 99104). Oxford, UK: Academic Press.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Miller, G.A. (1962). Psychology: The science of mental life. New York, NY: Adams, Bannister, & Cox.

  • Miller, N.E. (1959). Liberalization of basic S-R concepts: Extensions to conflict behavior, motivation, and social learning. In S. Koch (Ed.), Psychology: A study of a science (Vol. 2, pp. 196292). New York, NY: McGraw-Hill.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Milner, A.D., & Goodale, M. (1995). The visual brain in action. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

  • Mitchell, A.S., Baxter, M.G., & Gaffan, D. (2007). Dissociable performance on scene learning and strategy implementation after lesions to magnocellular mediodorsal thalamic nucleus. The Journal of Neuroscience, 27(44), 1188811895. PubMed ID: 17978029 doi:10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1835-07.2007

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Morsella, E. (2005). The function of phenomenal states: Supramodular interaction theory. Psychological Review, 112(4), 10001021. PubMed ID: 16262477 doi:10.1037/0033-295X.112.4.1000

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Morsella, E., & Bargh, J.A. (2011). Unconscious action tendencies: Sources of ‘un-integrated’ action. In J.T. Cacioppo & J. Decety (Eds.), The Oxford handbook of social neuroscience (pp. 335347). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Morsella, E., Godwin, C.A., Jantz, T.K., Krieger, S.C., & Gazzaley, A. (2016). Homing in on consciousness in the nervous system: An action-based synthesis [Target article]. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 39, 117.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Morsella, E., Gray, J.R., Krieger, S.C., & Bargh, J.A. (2009a). The essence of conscious conflict: Subjective effects of sustaining incompatible intentions. Emotion, 9(5), 717728. doi:10.1037/a0017121

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Morsella, E., & Krauss, R.M. (2004). The role of gestures in spatial working memory and speech. American Journal of Psychology, 117(3), 411424. PubMed ID: 15457809 doi:10.2307/4149008

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Morsella, E., Lanska, M., Berger, C.C., & Gazzaley, A. (2009c). Indirect cognitive control through top-down activation of perceptual symbols. European Journal of Social Psychology, 39(7), 11731177. doi:10.1002/ejsp.679

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Morsella, E., & Miozzo, M. (2002). Evidence for a cascade model of lexical access in speech production. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 28(3), 555563. PubMed ID: 12018507

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Morsella, E., & Poehlman, T.A. (2013). The inevitable contrast: Conscious versus unconscious processes in action control. Frontiers in Psychology, 4, 590. PubMed ID: 24058351 doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00590

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Morsella, E., Wilson, L.E., Berger, C.C., Honhongva, M., Gazzaley, A., & Bargh, J.A. (2009b). Subjective aspects of cognitive control at different stages of processing. Attention, Perception, & Psychophysics, 71(8), 18071824. doi:10.3758/APP.71.8.1807

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Mudrik, L., Faivre, N., & Koch, C. (2014). Information integration without awareness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 18(9), 488496. PubMed ID: 24933626 doi:10.1016/j.tics.2014.04.009

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Munakata, Y., Herd, S.A., Chatham, C.H., Depue, B.E., Banich, M.T., & O’Reilly, R.C. (2011). A unified framework for inhibitory control. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 15(10), 453459. PubMed ID: 21889391 doi:10.1016/j.tics.2011.07.011

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Muzur, A., Pace-Schott, E.F., & Hobson, J.A. (2002). The prefrontal cortex in sleep. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 6(11), 475481. PubMed ID: 12457899 doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(02)01992-7

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • O’Reilly, R.C. (2010). The what and how of prefrontal cortical organization. Trends in Neurosciences, 33(8), 355361. PubMed ID: 20573407 doi:10.1016/j.tins.2010.05.002

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pinker, S. (1997). How the mind works. New York, NY: Norton.

  • Prinz, J. (2007). The intermediate level theory of consciousness. In M. Velmans & S. Schneider (Eds.), The Blackwell companion to consciousness (pp. 248260). Malden, MA: Blackwell.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Prinz, W. (2003). How do we know about our own actions? In S. Maasen, W. Prinz, & G. Roth (Eds.), Voluntary action: Brains, minds, and sociality (pp. 2133). London, UK: Oxford University Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Prinz, W. (2012). Open minds: The social making of agency and intentionality. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  • Prinz, W., Aschersleben, G., & Koch, I. (2009). Cognition and action. In E. Morsella, J.A. Bargh, & P.M. Gollwitzer (Eds.), Oxford handbook of human action (pp. 3571). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Proctor, R.W., & Vu, K.-P.L. (2010). Action selection. In I.B. Weiner & E. Craighead (Eds.), The Corsini encyclopedia of psychology (Vol. 1, pp. 2022). Hoboken, NJ: John Wiley.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Pylyshyn, Z.W. (1984). Computation and cognition: Toward a foundation for cognitive science. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  • Questienne, L., Atas, A., Burle, B., & Gevers, W. (2018). Objectifying the subjective: Building blocks of metacognitive experiences in conflict tasks. Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 147(1), 125131. doi:10.1037/xge0000370

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rolls, E.T., Judge, S.J., & Sanghera, M. (1977). Activity of neurons in the inferotemporal cortex of the alert monkey. Brain Research, 130(2), 229238. PubMed ID: 406968 doi:10.1016/0006-8993(77)90272-4

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rosenbaum, D.A. (2002). Motor control. In H. Pashler (Series Ed.) & S. Yantis (Vol. Ed.), Stevens’ handbook of experimental psychology: Sensation and perception (3rd ed., Vol. 1, pp. 315339). New York, NY: Wiley.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Roser, M., & Gazzaniga, M.S. (2004). Automatic brains–interpretive minds. Current Directions in Psychological Science, 13(2), 5659. doi:10.1111/j.0963-7214.2004.00274.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rumelhart, D.E., McClelland, J.L., & the PDP Research Group. (1986). Parallel distributed processing: Explorations in the microstructure of cognition (Vols. 1 & 2). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Rumelhart, D.E., & Norman, D.A. (1988). Representation in memory. In R. Atkinson, D. Luce, & G. Lindzey (Eds.), Revised Stevens handbook of experimental psychology (pp. 511587). New York, NY: Wiley.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Schacter, D.L. (1996). Searching for memory: The brain, the mind, and the past. New York, NY: Basic Books.

  • Scott, M. (2013). Corollary discharge provides the sensory content of inner speech. Psychological Science, 24(9), 18241830. PubMed ID: 23846719 doi:10.1177/0956797613478614

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Searle, J.R. (2000). Consciousness. Annual Review of Neurosciences, 23, 557578. doi:10.1146/annurev.neuro.23.1.557

  • Simons, D.J., & Levin, D.T. (1997). Change blindness. Trends in Cognitive Sciences, 1(7), 261267. PubMed ID: 21223921 doi:10.1016/S1364-6613(97)01080-2

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Simpson, G.G. (1949). The meaning of evolution. New Haven, CT: Yale University Press.

  • Skinner, B.F. (1953). Science and human behavior. New York, NY: Macmillan.

  • Spear, N.E., & Campbell, B.A. (1979). Ontogeny of learning and memory. New York, NY: Lawrence Erlbaum.

  • Squire, L.R. (1987). Memory and brain. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

  • Stevens, S.S. (1975). Psychophysics: Introduction to its perceptual, neural, and social prospects. New York, NY: Wiley.

  • Stroop, J.R. (1935). Studies of interference in serial verbal reactions. Journal of Experimental Psychology, 18(6), 643662. doi:10.1037/h0054651

  • Stuss, D.T., & Anderson, V. (2004). The frontal lobes and theory of mind: Developmental concepts from adult focal lesion research. Brain & Cognition, 55(1), 6983. PubMed ID: 15134844 doi:10.1016/S0278-2626(03)00271-9

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Suzuki, T., Itoh, S., Arai, N., Kouno, M., Noguchi, M., Takatsu, M., & Takeda, K. (2012). Ambient echolalia in a patient with germinoma around the bilateral ventriculus lateralis: A case report. Neurocase, 18(4), 330335. PubMed ID: 22117108 doi:10.1080/13554794.2011.608364

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Taylor, J.A., & Ivry, R.B. (2013). Implicit and explicit processes in motor learning. In W. Prinz, M. Beisert, & A. Herwig (Eds.), Action science (pp. 6387). Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Thagard, P., & Stewart, T.C. (2014). Two theories of consciousness: Semantic pointer competition vs. information integration. Consciousness and Cognition, 30, 7390. PubMed ID: 25160821 doi:10.1016/j.concog.2014.07.001

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Titchener, E.B. (1896). An outline of psychology. New York, NY: Macmillan.

  • Tortora, G.J. (1994). Introduction to the human body: The essentials of anatomy and physiology (3rd ed.). New York, NY: Harper Collins.

  • Tsotsos, J.K. (1995). Behaviorist intelligence and the scaling problem. Artificial Intelligence, 75(2), 135160. doi:10.1016/0004-3702(94)00019-W

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Tsotsos, J.K. (2011). A computational perspective on visual attention. Cambridge, MA: MIT Press.

  • Vroomen, J., & de Gelder, B. (2003). Visual motion influences the contingent auditory motion aftereffect. Psychological Science, 14(4), 357361. PubMed ID: 12807410 doi:10.1111/1467-9280.24431

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Wegner, D.M. (1989). White bears and other unwanted thoughts. New York, NY: Viking/Penguin.

  • Weiskrantz, L. (1992). Unconscious vision: The strange phenomenon of blindsight. The Sciences, 35, 2328.

  • Weiskrantz, L. (1997). Consciousness lost and found: A neuropsychological exploration. New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

  • Westwood, D.A. (2009). The visual control of object manipulation. In E. Morsella, J.A. Bargh, & P.M. Gollwitzer (Eds.), Oxford handbook of human action (pp. 88103). New York, NY: Oxford University Press.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • White, N.A., Velasquez, A.G., & Morsella, E. (2018). Mary had a little . . . : Involuntary music imagery and memory retrieval. Poster presented at the Annual Convention of the Association for Psychological Science, San Francisco, CA.

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Yamadori, A. (1997). Body awareness and its disorders. In M. Ito, Y. Miyashita, Y., & E.T. Rolls (Eds.), Cognition, computation, and consciousness (pp. 169176). Washington, DC: American Psychological Association.

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Yankulova, J.K., & Morsella, E. (2020). Conscious contents: Their unanalyzable, arbitrary, and unarbitrary properties. Cognitive Neuropsychology. Advance online publication. doi:10.1080/02643294.2020.1728242

    • Crossref
    • PubMed
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • Yates, J. (1985). The content of awareness is a model of the world. Psychological Review, 92(2), 249284. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.92.2.249

All Time Past Year Past 30 Days
Abstract Views 4564 1734 82
Full Text Views 27 0 0
PDF Downloads 25 1 0