Subjective well-being (SWB) and its relationship with physical activity have not been systematically investigated in older Chinese people. This study explored these issues using qualitative interviews with a purposive sample of 23 community-dwelling Chinese older adults (age 55–78 y, 12 women); 16 were physically active and 7 physically inactive. Using cross-case analyses, 7 dimensions of SWB emerged: physical, psychological, developmental, material, spiritual, sociopolitical, and social. Although elements of SWB may be shared across cultures, specific distinctions were identified. Active respondents reported the unique contributions of physical activity to the physical, psychological, developmental, and social elements of SWB. The findings suggest that physical activity could enhance the quality of life in Chinese older adults.
Search Results
Dimensions of Subjective Well-Being and Effects of Physical Activity in Chinese Older Adults
Po-Wen Ku, Jim McKenna, and Kenneth R. Fox
Force and Accuracy Throws by Older Adults: II
Kathleen Williams, Kathleen Haywood, and Ann VanSant
Older adults were tested to clarify findings of an earlier examination of movement responses to shifting task requirements (Williams et al., 1993). Eleven participants (average age = 77 years) were evaluated on form and velocity as they performed overarm throws for force and accuracy. Significant gender and force-accuracy differences occurred for resultant velocity. Although no statistically significant differences occurred for force-accuracy comparisons of movement form, there were trends toward change in most movement components. Additionally, many individuals displayed change in one or more components as they shifted from force to accuracy throws. Results of this study point to the importance of examining developmental status and task requirements simultaneously.
An Argument for a Developmental Approach in Studying Older Adults’ Physical Activity
Diane E. Whaley
Force and Accuracy Throws by Older Adult Performers
Kathleen Williams, Kathleen Haywood, and Ann VanSant
Older adults threw tennis balls for force and accuracy to examine their adaptability to different task demands. Twenty-one (13 women, 8 men) participants were videotaped as they performed five force and five accuracy throws. The developmental level of each throw was determined; resultant ball velocities also were examined. Roberton’s (1977, 1978) movement components were used in the former analysis. The typical pattern of gender differences occurred for both movement component and velocity measures. Men performed at higher levels than women. Only minor force versus accuracy differences were found in the movement patterns used by either men or women; none of these differences were significant. Clear task differences occurred for ball velocities. Men’s forceful throws were faster than those for accuracy; women’s throws did not differ for the two tasks. The generally lower developmental level of women’s throws accounted for gender differences in velocity. Insufficient task differences may explain the lack of clear contrast in movement patterns.
Age, Gender, and Flexibility Differences in Tennis Serving among Experienced Older Adults
Kathleen M. Haywood and Kathleen Williams
This study examined tennis serving in older adult tennis players. Twenty-two older adults, divided into younger and older halves, were videotaped serving five “first” serves. Dominant shoulder flexibility also was measured. From the videotape, servers were classified into developmental levels and their resultant ball impact velocity was calculated. An Age × Gender (2 × 2) mixed model MANOVA yielded no significant differences between the age groups or between men and women in flexibility, ball impact velocity, or movement pattern. A few combinations of the developmental levels of elbow and forearm/racket action were used by the majority of servers. Regular practice might consolidate older adults in these attractor movement patterns, making them more resilient to change than with less practiced skills. These results suggest practice in older adulthood favorably affects performance by resulting in consistency of movement pattern and maintenance of movement pattern, flexibility, and ball impact speed.
Physical Activity and Dimensions of Subjective Well-Being in Older Adults
Aphrodite Stathi, Kenneth R. Fox, and James McKenna
Using a qualitative approach, the dimensions of subjective well-being of active older adults were outlined and ways identified through which they might be influenced by participation in physical activities. One-to-one and group interviews were used to collect the data. Using cross-case analysis, 17 main themes were identified. The following main dimensions emerged: developmental, material, physical, mental, and social well-being. The findings indicated that physical activity influences all dimensions of the subjective well-being of older adults, with the exception of material well-being. Physical activity appears to contribute to the mental health of older adults through maintenance of a busy and active life, mental alertness, positive attitude toward life and avoidance of stress, negative function, and isolation. The complexity of subjective well-being and the multiple roles of physical activity stress the need to extend qualitative research to sedentary older adults and the institutionalized elderly to explore the relationship between well-being and physical activity in later life.
For the Love of the Game: An Exploration of the Experiences and Psychosocial Development of Women 70 Years of Age and Older Who Play Volleyball
Jessica Brooke Kirby and Mary Ann Kluge
across the lifespan, and how the developmental experiences of midlife athletes might differ from those in later life. Considering how sport can offer access to resources for positive development, looking to development models used in youth sport may be particularly salient for older women, many of whom
Is There an Association Between Self-Reported Physical Activity and Self-Rated Vision Over Time? Results From the Irish Longitudinal Study of Ageing
Ilona I. McMullan, Brendan P. Bunting, Lee Smith, Ai Koyanagi, and Mark A. Tully
.K. ( 2013 ). Dealing with missing data in developmental research . Child Development Perspectives, 7, 27 – 31 . doi:10.1111/cdep.12008 10.1111/cdep.12008 Franco , M.R. , Tong , A. , Howard , K. , Sherrington , C. , Ferreira , P.H. , Pinto , R.Z. , & Ferreira , M.L. ( 2015 ). Older
The Use of Reference Values for the Timed Up and Go Test Applied in Multiple Scenarios?
Caroline Oliveira Gois, Alana Lalucha de Andrade Guimarães, Miburge Bolívar Gois Júnior, and Vitor Oliveira Carvalho
. Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 47 ( 8 ), 518 – 24 . 10.1111/j.1469-8749.2005.tb01185.x Yuksel , E. , Kalkan , S. , Cekmece , S. , Unver , B. , & Karatosun , V. ( 2017 ). Assessing minimal detectable changes and test-retest reliability of the timed up and go test and the 2-minute walk
Effects of Acute Exercise on Memory Performance in Middle-Aged and Older Adults
Jennifer L. Etnier, Jarod C. Vance, and Aiko Ueno
.G. , Torres-Trejo , F. , Gómez-Fernández , T. , … Gómez-Melgarejo , S. ( 2013 ). The rate of source memory decline across the adult life span . Developmental Psychology, 49 ( 5 ), 973 – 985 . doi:10.1037/a0028894 10.1037/a0028894 Chang , Y.K. , Labban , J.D. , Gapin , J.I. , & Etnier , J