Considering a Holistic Focus of Attention as an Alternative to an External Focus

in Journal of Motor Learning and Development
Restricted access

Purchase article

USD $24.95

Student 1 year subscription

USD $41.00

1 year subscription

USD $55.00

Student 2 year subscription

USD $79.00

2 year subscription

USD $103.00

Empirical findings consistently suggest that an external focus of attention leads to superior performance when compared to an internal focus by encouraging more automatic processing. However, for certain skills (e.g., gymnastics routines) it can be challenging to identify a meaningful external focus cue. A related line of research suggests that focusing on the general feeling of a movement (i.e., holistic focus) may also be useful in avoiding conscious control of movements. The purpose of this study was to determine how an internal focus (INT), external focus (EXT), and holistic focus (HOL) impact the performance of a standing long jump. Participants (N = 27) completed two baseline jumps followed by two jumps in each focus condition. Jump distance was analyzed in a 6 (Order) × 4 (Focus) mixed ANOVA. Results indicated a significant main effect of focus (p < .001), with EXT and HOL resulting in farther jumps than INT and baseline conditions (p-values < .05). EXT and HOL did not differ significantly from each other, and INT did not differ significantly from baseline. The findings suggest that a holistic focus can be another effective means of avoiding conscious control of movement when an external focus is not practical or desired.

Becker and Georges are with the School of Health Promotion and Kinesiology, Texas Woman's University, Denton, TX. Aiken is with the Department of Kinesiology and Dance, New Mexico State University, Las Cruces, NM.

Address author correspondence to Kevin A. Becker at Kbecker1@twu.edu.
Journal of Motor Learning and Development

Article Sections

References

  • AbdollahipourR.WulfG.PsottaR. & Palomo NietoM. (2015). Performance of gymnastics skill benefits from an external focus of attention. Journal of Sports Sciences 33(17) 18071813. PubMed ID: 25774536 doi:10.1080/02640414.2015.1012102

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BaumeisterR.F. (1984). Choking under pressure: Self-consciousness and paradoxical effects of incentives on skillful performance. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology 46(3) 610620. PubMed ID: 6707866 doi:10.1037/0022-3514.46.3.610

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BeckerK.A. & SmithP.J. (2015). Attentional focus effects in standing long jump performance: Influence of a broad and narrow internal focus. Journal of Strength & Conditioning Research 2917801783. PubMed ID: 26102255 doi:10.1519/JSC.0000000000000825

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BeilockS.L.CarrT.H.MacMahonC. & StarkesJ.L. (2002). When paying attention becomes counterproductive: Impact of divided versus skill-focused attention on novice and experienced performance of sensorimotor skills. Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied 8(1) 6. PubMed ID: 12009178 doi:10.1037/1076-898X.8.1.6

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BernierM.TrottierC.ThienotE. & FournierJ. (2016). An investigation of attentional foci and their temporal patterns: A naturalistic study in expert figure skaters. The Sport Psychologist 30(3) 256266. doi:10.1123/tsp.2013-0076

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • BorgG.A. (1982). Psychophysical bases of perceived exertion. Medicine & Science in Sports & Exercise 14(5) 377381. PubMed ID: 7154893 doi:10.1249/00005768-198205000-00012

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • EmanuelM.JarusT. & BartO. (2008). Effect of focus of attention and age on motor acquisition, retention, and transfer: A randomized trial. Physical Therapy 88(2) 251260. PubMed ID: 18042657 doi:10.2522/ptj.20060174

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • FairbrotherJ.T.PostP.G. & WhalenS.J. (2016). Self-reported responses to player profile questions show consistency with the use of complex attentional strategies by expert horseshoe pitchers. Frontiers in Psychology 71028. PubMed ID: 27458419 doi:10.3389/fpsyg.2016.01028

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • HaffG.G. & TriplettN.T. (Eds.). (2015). Essentials of strength training and conditioning (4th ed.). Champaign, IL: Human Kinetics.

  • LabanR. & LawrenceF.C. (1947). Effort. London, UK: Macdonald & Evans.

  • LawrenceG.P.GottwaldV.M.HardyJ. & KhanM.A. (2011). Internal and external focus of attention in a novice form sport. Research Quarterly for Exercise and Sport 82(3) 431441. PubMed ID: 21957701 doi:10.1080/02701367.2011.10599775

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MarchantD.C.CloughP.J.CrawshawM. & LevyA. (2009). Novice motor skill performance and task experience is influenced by attentional focusing instructions and instruction preferences. International Journal of Sport and Exercise Psychology 7(4) 488502. doi:10.1080/1612197X.2009.9671921

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MastersR.S. (1992). Knowledge, knerves and know-how: The role of explicit versus implicit knowledge in the breakdown of a complex motor skill under pressure. British Journal of Psychology 83(3) 343358. doi:10.1111/j.2044-8295.1992.tb02446.x

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • McNevinN.H.SheaC.H. & WulfG. (2003). Increasing the distance of an external focus of attention enhances learning. Psychological Research 672229. PubMed ID: 12589447 doi:10.1007/s00426-002-0093-6

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MullenR.FaullA.JonesE.S. & KingstonK. (2015). Evidence for the effectiveness of holistic process goals for learning and performance under pressure. Psychology of Sport and Exercise 174044. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2014.11.003

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • MullenR. & HardyL. (2010). Conscious processing and the process goal paradox. Journal of Sport & Exercise Psychology 32(3) 275297. PubMed ID: 20587818 doi:10.1123/jsep.32.3.275

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • PooltonJ.M.MastersR.S.W. & MaxwellJ.P. (2006). The influence of analogy learning on decision-making in table tennis: Evidence from behavioural data. Psychology of Sport and Exercise 7(6) 677688. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2006.03.005

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • PorterJ.M.AntonP.M. & WuW.F. (2012). Increasing the distance of an external focus of attention enhances standing long jump performance. Journal of Strength & Conditioning Research 26(9) 23892393. PubMed ID: 22067252 doi:10.1519/JSC.0b013e31823f275c

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • PorterJ.M.OstrowskiE.J.NolanR.P. & WuW.F. (2010). Standing long-jump performance is enhanced when using an external focus of attention. Journal of Strength & Conditioning Research 2417461750. PubMed ID: 20543731 doi:10.1519/JSC.0b013e3181df7fbf

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • PrinzW. (1997). Perception and action planning. European Journal of Cognitive Psychology 9(2) 129154. doi:10.1080/713752551

  • ThomasS.ReadingJ. & ShephardR.J. (1992). Revision of the physical activity readiness Questionnaire (PAR-Q). Canadian Journal of Sport Sciences 17(4) 338345. PubMed ID: 1330274

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WeissS.M.ReberA.S. & OwenD.R. (2008). The locus of focus: The effect of switching from a preferred to a non-preferred focus of attention. Journal of Sports Sciences 26(10) 10491057. PubMed ID: 18608831 doi:10.1080/02640410802098874

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WrisbergC.A. (2007). An applied sport psychological perspective on the relative merits of an external and internal focus of attention. In E.-J. Hossner&N. Wenderoth (Eds.) Gabrielle Wulf on attentional focus and motor learning [Target article]. E-Journal Bewegung und Training 15354. Retrieved from http://www.ejournal-but.de

    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WuW.F.PorterJ.M. & BrownL.E. (2012). Effect of attentional focus strategies on peak force and performance in the standing long jump. Journal of Strength & Conditioning Research 2612261231. PubMed ID: 22082793 doi:10.1519/JSC.0b013e318231ab61

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WulfG. (2013). Attentional focus and motor learning: A review of 15 years. International Review of Sport and Exercise Psychology 677104. doi:10.1080/1750984X.2012.723728

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WulfG.McNevinN. & SheaC.H. (2001). The automaticity of complex motor skill learning as a function of attentional focus. The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology: Section A 5411431154. doi:10.1080/713756012

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation
  • WulfG. & PrinzW. (2001). Directing attention to movement effects enhances learning: A review. Psychonomic Bulletin & Review 8648660. PubMed ID: 11848583 doi:10.3758/BF03196201

    • Crossref
    • Search Google Scholar
    • Export Citation

Article Metrics

All Time Past Year Past 30 Days
Abstract Views 86 86 80
Full Text Views 2 2 2
PDF Downloads 1 1 1

Altmetric Badge

PubMed

Google Scholar