In order to help people behave more healthily, intervention science has advocated a theory-driven approach of behavior change.1 This perspective considers that interventions can change behavior effectively, provided that they target factors with proven causal links to the behavior.2 Based on behavior change models (eg, Theory of Planned Behavior3,4; Health Action Process Approach5; Self-Determination Theory6; Dual-Process Approach7) several psychological factors have been identified as potential targets of intervention, such as conscious beliefs (eg, self-efficacy), motivations (eg, autonomous and controlled), self-regulation strategies (eg, planning), or automatic constructs (eg, implicit affect).
Although a theory-driven approach is essential to improve the quality of behavioral interventions, important caveats remain. Notably, the predictive validity of models and the effectiveness of interventions differ between populations. Recent meta-analyses suggest that intention relates to health behaviors more strongly in nonclinical populations than in clinical ones,8,9 and in the most educated samples.10 Interventions also produce stronger effects in the most educated people, as indicated by a meta-analysis of 123 interventions based on the Theory of Planned behavior,11 and a meta-analysis of digital physical activity interventions.12 A systematic review of 24 interventions in nonclinical older adults even suggests that self-regulation techniques (eg, goal setting, coping planning, self-monitoring of behavior) have deleterious effects on their self-efficacy and physical activity,13 whereas the same techniques seem beneficial in healthy younger adults.14,15 In sum, the average effectiveness of interventions hides large interindividual differences depending on the characteristics of the audience such as clinical status, educational level, or age.
These results are alarming. Chronic disease patients and older adults are among the most physically inactive groups,16 and low socioeconomic status people are the most likely to adopt unhealthy behaviors more generally.17 As such, intervention science faces a hazardous paradox: on the one hand, these populations are those for whom adoption of healthy behaviors is most urgent; on the other hand, behavior change models are less predictive, and interventions less successful, in these populations. A paradox that may ultimately impede intervention science to bridge the growing health gap between advantaged and disadvantaged individuals.18
An Undue Emphasis on the Cognitions of Fit and Young Individuals From High-Income Countries
At least 4 reasons may explain this paradox. First, research mostly investigates what causes behavior (eg, individual cognitions) and how to change it (eg, behavior change techniques), at the expense of examining among whom and under what conditions these factors and techniques do or do not predict behavior (eg, moderating effects of sociodemographic or contextual factors). As an illustration, most recent meta-analyses of behavior change models and interventions did not (or could not) include participants’ socioeconomic background or clinical status as potential moderators.8,9,19–26 This prevents intervention scientists to examine the conditions of application of these models.
Second, behavior change models put an undue emphasis on individual cognitions. For example, much of the current debate on the “intention–behavior gap” focuses on the cognitive factors that moderate this gap.27 Yet, behaviors rely on a wider range of determinants: at the individual level, they may include biological (eg, health status, body mass index and fitness level) and sociodemographic characteristics (eg, age, sex, education); at the contextual level, precursors of behavior can be found in the macro-environment (eg, transport and health policy, social norms) and in the micro-environment (eg, social, built, or natural).16 Crucially, factors from different levels may interact in a complex manner. For example, increasing access to recreational facilities may be necessary but insufficient to increase physical activity, as its influence may conjointly depend on the motivation to exercise.28 Conversely, successfully translating intention to eat healthy into action may depend on the accessibility of vegetables in the neighborhood.29 In sum, the current focus on individual cognitions prevents a complete understanding of the multifactorial and intricated causes of health behaviors.
Third, research on health behavior change is concerned with sampling issues. Studies are rarely conducted on populations in situations of vulnerability (eg, patients, older adults, low-income individuals), but rather on young and healthy people (eg, see some initiatives like the Swiss National Center of Competence in Research LIVES, or the Poverty Action Lab). A look at the 95 studies on the Health Action Process Approach applied to multiple health behaviors meta-analyzed by Zhang et al9 reveals that only 8% of the studies were conducted on older adults, relative to 85% on young and middle-aged adults. Likewise, 31% of studies were conducted on clinical populations versus 69% on nonclinical ones. In addition, most studies include samples from high-income countries. For example, 88% of the 265 studies on Self-Determination Theory meta-analyzed by Vasconcellos et al26 were conducted in Europe and North America, only 8% in Asia, 4% in Australia and Oceania, and no study in South America or Africa. Similarly, an analysis of the articles published from 2014 to 2018 in one of the top-tier journals of the American Psychological Association, Health Psychology, showed that 64% of the samples came from the United States, 21% from English-speaking countries, 10% from Europe, and only 5% from the rest of the world.30 This sampling problem is not specific to intervention science and concerns behavioral sciences more generally. People from Western, Educated, Industrialized, Rich and Democratic (WEIRD) societies represent 80% of samples in psychology31 and biomedical sciences,32 while they account for only 12% of the worldwide population. This overrepresentation leads scientists to mistakenly conclude from the universality of phenomena that are yet sensitive to context, and individuals’ characteristics, as observed in disease etiology.33 Intervention science seems far from immune to such drawback. Recursively, this sampling bias could prevent from examining among whom and under what conditions models are valid and interventions effective, as it may result in insufficient variability in the factors of interest.34
Fourth, the WEIRD bias also extends to researchers30 and journal editors.35,36 For example, 64% of first authors of articles published in Health Psychology from 2014 to 2018 lived in the United States, 23% in English-speaking countries, 11% in Europe, and only 1% in the rest of the world.30 This structural ethnocentrism is problematic as researchers may tend to apply their own system of thoughts and values to the way they conceive and conduct research.37 For example, Roberts et al38 showed that White editors-in-chief of journals in cognitive, developmental, and social psychology, publish less articles highlighting race than non-White editors-in-chief. Likewise, White authors employ fewer non-White participants in their samples than non-White authors. As such, an overrepresentation of researchers from high socioeconomic background or high-income countries could partly explain why populations in situation of vulnerability are underrepresented in intervention science.
How to Address the Undue Emphasis on the Fit and Young Individuals From WEIRD Countries? We Need Big Ideas to Solve Big Problems
It has become urgent to change the way intervention scientists conceive and conduct research. At the conceptual level, we need to develop theories (1) that not only model more thoroughly how characteristics of the audience (related to sociodemographic characteristics and health) and the context (at the micro and macro level) may act as boundary limits of theory application and intervention effectiveness, but also (2) consider context and audience as variables of key importance in predicting behaviors.
Toward a Thorough Conceptualization of the Role of Context and Audience in Health Behaviors
To do so, one solution is to enrich behavior change models with theoretical approaches that take into account the context. For example, the socioecological perspective39 or the Capability Opportunity Motivation and Behavior (COM-B) model40 consider both individual and contextual factors of behavior. However, how these different strata of factors interact with each other remains largely unknown (but see Rhodes et al41) and interventions that simultaneously tap onto these different strata are rare.42
In the same vein, integrating findings from epidemiology and social psychology could help to better understand how the context and audience moderate interventions’ effectiveness. Some epidemiologists proposed that such effectiveness depends on the extent to which they rely on participants’ personal resources (ie, personal motivation and ability to engage with the intervention).43 Specifically, interventions that require participants to use personal resources (eg, information campaigns that imply active seeking for knowledge) are likely to reinforce socioeconomic health inequalities by further excluding disadvantaged populations. In contrast, these authors hypothesize that interventions that do not require personal resources, such as “nudging” interventions—which target instead the micro-environment—are more likely to be equitable. Although interesting, such assertion deserves further empirical support, as the effectiveness of nudge interventions.44 Similarly, research from the social psychology of stereotypes suggests that interventions requiring self-control resources (ie, energy to self-regulate) could be less effective in populations in situations of vulnerability, because their self-control resources are likely to be low.45 Indeed, these populations have to cope with the detrimental effects (eg, stress) induced by everyday stigmatization (eg, related to age, weight, health, socioeconomic status). This may tax self-control resources and, therefore, decrease their availability for other resource-demanding activities that are typically involved by health behavior change, such as planning one’s physical activity.46
Other approaches may help to provide a more central place to the context and audience characteristics in health behavior change modelization. For example, researchers in behavioral economics47 have conceptualized in a fine-grained manner how poverty may undermine cognitive abilities and motivation, by taxing cognitive resources or keeping people away from focusing on long-term goals. Such impact could have in turn deleterious consequences on health behaviors. Adopting a life course perspective could also allow an in-depth analysis of the consequences of (unprivileged) circumstances throughout life. Indeed, vulnerabilities may accumulate over time and contextual factors may shape the way individuals react to major life events (eg, birth, divorce, moving, retirement), which can in turn have a strong influence on health behaviors across life.48 For example, after a cardiovascular event, people from low socioeconomic status were less likely to change their health behaviors (eg, stop smoking, being more physically active) than people from higher socioeconomic status.49
In sum, combining behavior change models with other theoretical approaches could enrich our understanding of the health behavior determinants that are specific to populations in situations of vulnerability and that are likely to fluctuate across the life course. Ultimately, this would help to identify behavioral change techniques that are adapted to audience and context characteristics. This requires intervention scientists to develop larger time-windowed projects and interdisciplinary collaborations by cross-referencing approaches from different disciplines (eg, economics, epidemiology, geography, psychology, sociology). International collaborations (see for eg, the Psychological Accelerator initiative) or active collaborations with patients and members of the targeted public at all stages of the research project may also be fruitful to broaden researchers’ system of thought.
Toward More Rigorous Methodologies and Reporting of Samples Characteristics
At the methodological level, different propositions can be made in order to better evaluate for whom and when interventions are effective. A first solution is to systematically provide information on participants’ socioeconomic and demographic background (eg, education, income, occupation, zone of residence, country) in empirical reports of both interventional and observational studies. Currently, 1 in 4 studies in psychology provides no information on the samples used.33 In the health domain, a recent systematic review of weight management interventions in men indicates that 7 out of 36 studies did not report any socioeconomic information about their samples.50 Even if socioeconomic markers may not have the same meaning in different countries (given differences in income, school systems, etc), providing such information in a systematic manner would allow meta-analyses to examine whether the validity of behavior change models or the effectiveness of interventions are moderated by socioeconomic and demographic characteristics. Likewise, some researchers call for a more systematic reporting of authors’ own socioeconomic and cultural characteristics.37 They consider that this could allow to better evaluate how researchers’ characteristics shape their approach to research.
Second, better identifying for which populations interventions are effective or not can be done by using more rigorous or innovative designs. Randomized controlled trials are well adapted for this purpose; however, they are often underpowered. This prevents moderation analyses to be performed. Therefore, conducting highly powered randomized controlled trials is needed to better evaluate for whom and under what conditions interventions are effective. As access to participants may be difficult, especially when it comes to clinical populations or people living in disadvantaged socioeconomic conditions, alternative designs could be preferred. As an illustration, N-of-1 trials with intensive longitudinal studies (eg, using experience sampling methods) involve many repeated measurements taken on individuals,51 and could allow to provide new insights on the interactions between cognitions and characteristics of the context and audience at the within-person level (eg, how an increase in financial deprivation at the day-level widens the intention–action gap). Quasi-experimental studies comparing data from samples living in different environments (eg, in terms of access to recreation facilities) may also be helpful to examine interactions between environmental and personal factors.
Conclusion
Intervention scientists are well aware that changing behaviors is not an easy task, and this also applies to the way they do research. Replacing long-used research practices by novel ones may be time and energy-consuming. However, providing complete information on sample characteristics may be a small step for researchers, but can represent a big first step for the field. Increasing the diversity of sampled populations or integrating individual and contextual factors in a life-course perspective, can be done by combining expertise through collaborations with researchers from different disciplines and countries, and with participants of the targeted public (see Figure 1). Regardless the cost of these propositions, it becomes urgent to move toward a more sample-diversified and multifactorial intervention science. Its social utility and credibility depend on it.
—Schematic representation of the zeitgeist of intervention science and needed future directions. Note. The upper figure represents the current zeitgeist of research on behavioral interventions. The lower figure represents future directions that need to be taken to address issues related to the current undue emphasis on individual cognitions in FYI-WEIRD samples. FYI-WEIRD indicates Fit and Young Individuals from Western Educated Industrialized and Rich Democracies; RCT, randomized controlled trials; WEIRD, Western Educated Industrialized and Rich Democracies.
Citation: Journal of Physical Activity and Health 20, 6; 10.1123/jpah.2023-0072
Acknowledgments
Chalabaev is supported by the French National Research Agency in the framework of the Investissements d’Avenir Programme (ANR-15-IDEX-02) for the Sport Perf Health project.
References
- 1.↑
Araújo-Soares V, Hankonen N, Presseau J, Rodrigues A, Sniehotta FF. Developing behavior change interventions for self-management in chronic illness. Eur Psychol. 2018;24(1):7–25. PubMed ID: 31496632 doi:10.1027/1016-9040/a000330
- 2.↑
Sheeran P, Klein WMP, Rothman AJ. Health behavior change: moving from observation to intervention. Annu Rev Psychol. 2017;68(1):573–600. PubMed ID: 27618942 doi:10.1146/annurev-psych-010416-044007
- 3.↑
Ajzen I. The theory of planned behavior. Organ Behav Hum Decis Process. 1991;50(2):179–211. doi:10.1016/0749-5978(91)90020-T
- 4.↑
Ajzen I. The theory of planned behaviour: reactions and reflections. Psychol Health. 2011;26(9):1113–1127. PubMed ID: 21929476 doi:10.1080/08870446.2011.613995
- 5.↑
Schwarzer R, Luszczynska A. How to overcome health-compromising behaviors: the health action process approach. Eur Psychol. 2008;13(2):141–151. doi:10.1027/1016-9040.13.2.141
- 6.↑
Deci EL, Ryan RM. Self-determination theory: a macrotheory of human motivation, development, and health. Can Psychol. 2008;49(3):182–185. doi:10.1037/a0012801
- 7.↑
Hofmann W, Friese M, Wiers RW. Impulsive versus reflective influences on health behavior: a theoretical framework and empirical review. Health Psychol Rev. 2008;2(2):111–137. doi:10.1080/17437190802617668
- 8.↑
Choi Y, Yang SJ, Song HY. Effects of the variables related to the health action process approach model on physical activity: a systematic literature review and meta-analysis. J Korean Acad Community Health Nurs. 2018;29(3):359–370. doi:10.12799/jkachn.2018.29.3.359
- 9.↑
Zhang C-Q, Zhang R, Schwarzer R, Hagger M. A meta-analysis of the health action process approach. Health Psychol. 2019;38(7):623–637. PubMed ID: 30973747 doi:10.1037/hea0000728
- 10.↑
Schüz B, Li AS-W, Hardinge A, McEachan RRC, Conner M. Socioeconomic status as a moderator between social cognitions and physical activity: systematic review and meta-analysis based on the theory of planned behavior. Psychol Sport Exerc. 2017;30:186–195. doi:10.1016/j.psychsport.2017.03.004
- 11.↑
Steinmetz H, Knappstein M, Ajzen I, Schmidt P, Kabst R. How effective are behavior change interventions based on the theory of planned behavior? A three-level meta-analysis. Z Fur Psychol. 2016;224(3):216–233. doi:10.1027/2151-2604/a000255
- 12.↑
Western MJ, Armstrong MEG, Islam I, Morgan K, Jones UF, Kelson MJ. The effectiveness of digital interventions for increasing physical activity in individuals of low socioeconomic status: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Behav Nutr Phys Act. 2021;18(1):1–21. doi:10.1186/s12966-021-01218-4
- 13.↑
French DP, Olander EK, Chisholm A, Mc Sharry J. Which behaviour change techniques are most effective at increasing older adults’ self-efficacy and physical activity behaviour? A systematic review. Ann Behav Med. 2014;48(2):225–234. PubMed ID: 24648017 doi:10.1007/s12160-014-9593-z
- 14.↑
Michie S, Abraham C, Whittington C, McAteer J, Gupta S. Effective techniques in healthy eating and physical activity interventions: a meta-regression. Health Psychol. 2009;28(6):690–701. PubMed ID: 19916637 doi:10.1037/a0016136
- 15.↑
Williams SL, French DP. What are the most effective intervention techniques for changing physical activity self-efficacy and physical activity behaviour—and are they the same? Health Educ Res. 2011;26(2):308–322. PubMed ID: 21321008 doi:10.1093/her/cyr005
- 16.↑
Bauman AE, Reis RS, Sallis JF, et al. Correlates of physical activity: why are some people physically active and others not? Lancet. 2012;380(9838):258–271. doi:10.1016/S0140-6736(12)60735-1
- 17.↑
Petrovic D, de Mestral C, Bochud M, et al. The contribution of health behaviors to socioeconomic inequalities in health: a systematic review. Prev Med. 2018;113:15–31. PubMed ID: 29752959 doi:10.1016/j.ypmed.2018.05.003
- 18.↑
Tawiah R, Jagger C, Anstey KJ, Kiely KM. Lifecourse socioeconomic position and cohort differences in health expectancy in Australia: a longitudinal cohort study. Lancet Glob Health. 2022;7(4):e347–e355. PubMed ID: 35366409 doi:10.1016/S2468-2667(22)00026-3
- 19.↑
Cooke R, Dahdah M, Norman P, French DP. How well does the theory of planned behaviour predict alcohol consumption? A systematic review and meta-analysis. Health Psychol Rev. 2016;10(2):148–167. PubMed ID: 25089611 doi:10.1080/17437199.2014.947547
- 20.
Gillison FB, Rouse P, Standage M, Sebire SJ, Ryan RM. A meta-analysis of techniques to promote motivation for health behaviour change from a self-determination theory perspective. Health Psychol Rev. 2019;13(1):110–130. PubMed ID: 30295176 doi:10.1080/17437199.2018.1534071
- 21.
Hagger M, Cheung MW-L, Ajzen I, Hamilton K. Perceived behavioral control moderating effects in the theory of planned behavior: a meta-analysis. Health Psychol. 2022;41(2):155–167. PubMed ID: 35143225 doi:10.1037/hea0001153
- 22.
Lin N, Roberts KR. Using the theory of planned behavior to predict food safety behavioral intention: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Int J Hosp Manag. 2020;90:102612. doi:10.1016/j.ijhm.2020.102612
- 23.
McDermott MS, Oliver M, Simnadis T, et al. The theory of planned behaviour and dietary patterns: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Prev Med. 2015;81:150–156. https://ro.uow.edu.au/smhpapershttps://ro.uow.edu.au/smhpapers/3157
- 24.
Ntoumanis N, Ng JYY, Prestwich A, et al. A meta-analysis of self-determination theory-informed intervention studies in the health domain: effects on motivation, health behavior, physical, and psychological health. Health Psychol Rev. 2021;15(2):214–244. PubMed ID: 31983293 doi:10.1080/17437199.2020.1718529
- 25.
Sheeran P, Wright CE, Avishai A, et al. Self-determination theory interventions for health behavior change: meta-analysis and meta-analytic structural equation modeling of randomized controlled trials. J Consult Clin Psychol. 2020;88(8):726–737. PubMed ID: 32437175 doi:10.1037/ccp0000501
- 26.↑
Vasconcellos D, Parker P, Hilland T, et al. Self-determination theory applied to physical education: a systematic review and meta-analysis. J Educ Psychol. 2020;112(7):1444–1469. doi:10.1037/edu0000420
- 27.↑
Rhodes RE, Cox A, Sayar R. What predicts the physical activity intention–behavior gap? A systematic review. Ann Behav Med. 2022;56(1):1–20. PubMed ID: 34231844 doi:10.1093/abm/kaab044
- 28.↑
Giles-Corti B, Donovan RJ. The relative influence of individual, social and physical environment determinants of physical activity. Soc Sci Med. 2002;54(12):1793–1812. PubMed ID: 12113436 doi:10.1016/S0277-9536(01)00150-2
- 29.↑
Schulz AJ, Odoms-Young AM, Zenk SN, et al. Fruit and vegetable intake in African Americans income and store characteristics. Am J Prev Med. 2005;29(1):1–9. doi:10.1016/j.amepre.2005.03.002
- 30.↑
Thalmayer AG, Toscanelli C, Arnett JJ. The neglected 95% revisited: is American psychology becoming less American? Am Psychol. 2021;76(1):116–129. PubMed ID: 32271027 doi:10.1037/amp0000622
- 31.↑
Henrich J, Heine SJ, Norenzayan A. The weirdest people in the world? Behav Brain Sci. 2010;33(2–3):61–83. PubMed ID: 20550733 doi:10.1017/S0140525X0999152X
- 32.↑
Gurven MD, Lieberman DE. WEIRD bodies: mismatch, medicine and missing diversity. Evol Hum Behav. 2020;41(5):330–340. PubMed ID: 33100820 doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2020.04.001
- 33.↑
Apicella C, Norenzayan A, Henrich J. Beyond WEIRD: a review of the last decade and a look ahead to the global laboratory of the future. Evol Hum Behav. 2020;41(5):319–329. doi:10.1016/j.evolhumbehav.2020.07.015
- 34.↑
Ding D, Carson V, Hunter RF, et al. Science has no borders, so should scientific publishing: a position statement from the journal of physical activity and health. J Phys Act Health. 2022;19(12):809–810. PubMed ID: 36318917
- 35.↑
Altman M, Cohen PN. Openness and diversity in journal editorial boards. 2021. doi:10.31235/OSF.IO/4NQ97
- 36.↑
Lin Z, Li N. Global diversity of authors, editors, and journal ownership across subdisciplines of psychology: current state and policy Implications. Perspect Psychol Sci. Published online 2022. doi:10.1177/17456916221091831
- 37.↑
Brady LM, Fryberg SA, Shoda Y. Expanding the interpretive power of psychological science by attending to culture. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA. 2018;115(45):11406–11413. PubMed ID: 30397134 doi:10.1073/pnas.1803526115
- 38.↑
Roberts SO, Bareket-Shavit C, Dollins FA, Goldie PD, Mortenson E. Racial inequality in psychological research: trends of the past and recommendations for the future. Perspect Psychol Sci. 2020;15(6):1295–1309. PubMed ID: 32578504 doi:10.1177/1745691620927709
- 39.↑
McLeroy KR, Bibeau D, Steckler A, Glanz K. An ecology perspective on health promotion programs. Health Educ Q. 1988;15(4):351–377. PubMed ID: 3068205 doi:10.1177/109019818801500401
- 40.↑
Michie S, van Stralen MM, West R. The behaviour change wheel: a new method for characterising and designing behaviour change interventions. Implement Sci. 2011;6(1):42. doi:10.1186/1748-5908-6-42
- 41.↑
Rhodes RE, Saelens BE, Sauvage-Mar C. Understanding physical activity through interactions between the built environment and social cognition: a systematic review. Sports Med. 2018;48(8):1893–1912. PubMed ID: 29767384 doi:10.1007/s40279-018-0934-0
- 42.↑
Teran-Escobar C, Duché S, Bouscasse H, et al. InterMob: a 24-month randomised controlled trial comparing the effectiveness of an intervention including behavioural change techniques and free transport versus an intervention including air pollution awareness-raising on car use reduction among regular car users living in Grenoble, France. BMC Public Health. 2022;22(1):1–16. doi:10.1186/s12889-022-14099-4
- 43.↑
Adams J, Mytton O, White M, Monsivais P. Why are some population interventions for diet and obesity more equitable and effective than others? The role of individual agency. PLoS Med. 2016;13(4):e1001990. PubMed ID: 27046234 doi:10.1371/journal.pmed.1001990
- 44.↑
Marteau TM, Ogilvie D, Roland M, Suhrcke M, Kelly MP. Judging nudging: can nudging improve population health? BMJ. 2011;342(7791):263–265. doi:10.1136/BMJ.D228
- 45.↑
Chalabaev A, Mazéas A, Forestier C, Sniehotta F. “I am fatigued of being stigmatized”: on the need to investigate stigma-related barriers to physical activity. Movement & Sport Sciences—Science & Motricité. Published online 2023. doi:10.1051/sm/2022028
- 46.↑
Sjåstad H, Baumeister RF. The future and the will: planning requires self-control, and ego depletion leads to planning aversion. J Exp Soc Psychol. 2018;76:127–141. doi:10.1016/j.jesp.2018.01.005
- 48.↑
Cullati S, Kliegel M, Widmer E. Development of reserves over the life course and onset of vulnerability in later life. Nat Hum Behav. 2018;2(8):551–558. PubMed ID: 31209322 doi:10.1038/s41562-018-0395-3
- 49.↑
Chan RHM, Gordon NF, Chong A, Alter DA. Influence of socioeconomic status on lifestyle behavior modifications among survivors of acute myocardial infarction. Am J Card. 2008;102(12):1583–1588. PubMed ID: 19064009 doi:10.1016/j.amjcard.2008.08.022
- 50.↑
McDonald MD, Hunt K, Sivaramakrishnan H, et al. A systematic review examining socioeconomic factors in trials of interventions for men that report weight as an outcome. Obes Rev. 2022;23(7):e13436. PubMed ID: 35187778 doi:10.1111/obr.13436
- 51.↑
Perski O, Keller J, Kale D, et al. Understanding health behaviours in context: a systematic review and meta-analysis of ecological momentary assessment studies of five key health behaviours. Health Psychol Rev. 2022;16(4):576–601. PubMed ID: 35975950 doi:10.1080/17437199.2022.2112258